首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   82760篇
  免费   3303篇
  国内免费   26篇
  86089篇
  2020年   799篇
  2019年   1003篇
  2018年   1375篇
  2017年   1398篇
  2016年   1526篇
  2015年   1113篇
  2014年   1304篇
  2013年   6255篇
  2012年   2406篇
  2011年   2637篇
  2010年   1653篇
  2009年   1658篇
  2008年   2331篇
  2007年   2335篇
  2006年   2131篇
  2005年   1890篇
  2004年   1803篇
  2003年   1744篇
  2002年   1827篇
  2001年   2485篇
  2000年   2433篇
  1999年   1896篇
  1998年   985篇
  1997年   905篇
  1996年   899篇
  1995年   810篇
  1992年   1627篇
  1991年   1486篇
  1990年   1479篇
  1989年   1351篇
  1988年   1345篇
  1987年   1278篇
  1986年   1394篇
  1985年   1398篇
  1984年   1192篇
  1983年   1093篇
  1982年   790篇
  1981年   792篇
  1979年   1270篇
  1978年   951篇
  1975年   1038篇
  1974年   1166篇
  1973年   1271篇
  1972年   1038篇
  1971年   1009篇
  1970年   905篇
  1969年   946篇
  1968年   1190篇
  1967年   1068篇
  1966年   974篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
43.
44.
45.
46.
47.
The ability to make egocentric distance estimates of a single point source of light, seen in darkness and without the cues of changing size and luminance, was investigated in sixteen observers. The attenuation required to maintain constant luminance, when the target was viewed from different distances, was shown to follow the inverse square law providing the angle subtended by the light was less than 20 s arc. Distance changes were also simulated by means of a split mirror which produced vergence cues, or by test lenses to provide accommodation cues. Over the range 0.5 to 9.2 m distance estimates were surprisingly accurate, although there was some overestimation of near and underestimation of far distances. Most observers made good judgements when only convergence cues were varied, whereas no observers made consistently good judgements when only accommodation cues were varied. The difficulties are discussed in terms of the accommodation-convergence link. When distance was simulated by changing convergence and accommodation cues, estimates were not as good as when real distance was changed. Since good estimates were made with brief target exposures, these judgements were not based on subsequent convergence or accommodation changes. It is suggested that the metric or reference against which the apparently absolute judgements were made was the efferent demand signal associated with a 'resting' position of convergence in darkness.  相似文献   
48.
49.
50.
Four experiments were conducted to determine whether echoic memory plays a role in differences between good and poor readers. The first two experiments used a suffix procedure in which the subject is read a list of digits with either a tone control or the word go appended to the list. For lists that exceeded the length of the subjects memory span by one digit (i.e., that avoided ceiling effects), the poor readers showed a larger decrement in the suffix condition than did the good readers. The third experiment was directed at the question of whether the duration of echoic memory is different for good and poor readers. Children shadowed words presented to one ear at a rate determined to give 75-85% shadowing accuracy. The items presented to the nonattended ear were words and an occasional digit. At various intervals after the presentation of the digit, a light signaled that the subject was to cease shadowing and attempt to recall any digit that had occurred in the nonattended ear recently. Whereas good and poor readers recalled the digit equally if tested immediately after presentation, the poor readers showed a faster decline in recall of the digit as retention interval increased. A fourth experiment was conducted to determine whether the differences in echoic memory were specific to speech stimuli or occurred at a more basic level of aural persistence. Bursts of white noise were separated by 9-400 ms of silence and the subject was to say whether there were one or two sounds presented. There were no differences in detectability functions for good and poor readers.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号