全文获取类型
收费全文 | 11800篇 |
免费 | 173篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
11974篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 150篇 |
2019年 | 168篇 |
2018年 | 257篇 |
2017年 | 232篇 |
2016年 | 284篇 |
2015年 | 208篇 |
2014年 | 200篇 |
2013年 | 1054篇 |
2012年 | 425篇 |
2011年 | 475篇 |
2010年 | 277篇 |
2009年 | 317篇 |
2008年 | 379篇 |
2007年 | 438篇 |
2006年 | 350篇 |
2005年 | 339篇 |
2004年 | 350篇 |
2003年 | 334篇 |
2002年 | 317篇 |
2001年 | 321篇 |
2000年 | 303篇 |
1999年 | 243篇 |
1998年 | 161篇 |
1997年 | 132篇 |
1996年 | 138篇 |
1995年 | 121篇 |
1994年 | 130篇 |
1993年 | 108篇 |
1992年 | 186篇 |
1991年 | 162篇 |
1990年 | 168篇 |
1989年 | 130篇 |
1988年 | 112篇 |
1987年 | 134篇 |
1986年 | 141篇 |
1985年 | 142篇 |
1984年 | 128篇 |
1983年 | 134篇 |
1982年 | 84篇 |
1981年 | 88篇 |
1979年 | 133篇 |
1978年 | 113篇 |
1977年 | 89篇 |
1974年 | 105篇 |
1973年 | 113篇 |
1972年 | 84篇 |
1970年 | 84篇 |
1968年 | 114篇 |
1967年 | 85篇 |
1966年 | 95篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
21.
Single-subject statistics were used to analyze physiological data [electromyographic (EMG) temperature] recorded from two patients receiving computer-assisted biofeedback treatment in a clinic. When the data were analyzed by an ANOVA method or a regression technique, the results demonstrated that both patients learned to significantly reduce EMG levels. The rationale of using single-subject statistics is discussed, and the applicability of the statistical technique to the clinic population is illustrated. 相似文献
22.
Gloria S. Waters Margaret Bruck Mark Seidenberg 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1985,39(3):511-530
The use of spelling-sound information in both reading and spelling was evaluated by having children read and spell nonwords and five types of words that differed in terms of their regularity for reading and spelling. The subjects were grade 3 children who had been psychometrically defined as good readers and good spellers (“good”), good readers and poor spellers (“mixed”), or poor readers and poor spellers (“poor”). Results indicated that all children attempted to use spelling-sound correspondences in both reading and spelling, although children in both the mixed and the poor groups had weaker knowledge of these correspondences and were less systematic in their use of them. Furthermore, even though the children in the mixed group had been matched with children in the good group on reading comprehension, the number and type of errors made by the mixed subjects on both the reading and spelling tasks were more similar to those of the poor subjects than to those of the good subjects. 相似文献
23.
Sex Roles - Forty-eight toddler boys and girls, 18 to 36 months of age, were observed in play groups. The assertive acts of each child and the responses of peers and teachers were recorded. The... 相似文献
24.
Mark Bennett 《Current Psychology》1985,4(4):323-329
It was hypothesized (1) that with age children would rely increasingly upon information about intention, rather than overt
behavior, when attributing dispositional features to others; and (2) that, developmentally, “restricted” dispositional attributions
(i.e., those that assume the operation of highly specific situation-response relationships) would precede “generalized” dispositional
attributions (which assume the operation of a dispositional concept between broad classes of situations and responses). An
experiment is reported that provides support for the first, though not the second, of these hypotheses. 相似文献
25.
E. James Kehoe Constantine X. Poulos I. Gormezano 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1985,20(1):29-38
Two experiments examined appetitive differential conditioning of the rabbit’s jaw movement response (JMR) in a two-phase procedure. The first phase entailed reinforced training with one conditioned stimulus (CS+), and the second phase involved intermixed presentations of CS+ and an unreinforced stimulus (CS?). In Experiment 1, CS+ was a 600-Hz tone, and CS-was either a 660-,1,000-, or 2,100-Hz tone. In Experiment 2, the magnitude of the water unconditioned stimulus (US) paired with CS+ was either 1, 3, or 9 ml. The experiments revealed that 1) the level of responding to CS-rose for several days and then declined over the remainder of training; 2) the physical similarity between CS+ and CS?directly affected the level of responding to CS?but had no discernable effect on the level of responding to CS+ ; and 3) US magnitude positively affected the level of responding to CS + and, to a lesser extent, CS?. The results are discussed in terms of their implications for Spence’s gradient interaction theory and Pavlov’s external inhibition hypothesis. 相似文献
26.
R M Olig R D Staton W W Beatty H Wilson R I Biberdorf S G Hoag R A Brumback 《Perceptual and motor skills》1985,60(3):879-889
Plasma concentrations of imipramine and amitriptyline and their desmethylated metabolites were measured in 20 children being treated for major depressive illness 2 wk. and 5 to 10 wk. after achieving drug dosages of 2.25 mg/kg body weight. At 2 wk. all children had exhibited clinical improvement, but by 10 wk. 4 of the 10 children treated with imipramine and 5 of the 10 children treated with amitriptyline had experienced clinical relapse of depressive symptoms. Tricyclic antidepressant plasma concentrations and ratios were comparable in the subgroups of children who maintained their clinical improvement and those who relapsed. There was no evidence of a systematic decline in plasma tricyclic antidepressant concentrations in those children who relapsed. 相似文献
27.
R Witkowski E Ullrich P Pietsch K Weber K Heller T Losanowa I Nitz 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1985,37(5):255-261
We present four cases of Prader-Willi syndrome. Two of them have an abnormality of a chromosome 15, the other both show different chromosomal abnormalities. Translocations or deletions were found recently in the bands 15q11/12 in about 60% of the cases of Prader-Willi syndrome. The consequences for diagnosis, symptomatology and genetic counselling of the syndrome are discussed. 相似文献
28.
Lawrence J. Epstein Mitchel T. Taubman Dr. O. I. Lovaas 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1985,13(2):281-293
For four of six autistic children who underwent intensive behavioral treatment, the nature of their self-stimulatory behavior changed from initial lowlevel motor behaviors (such as rocking, spinning, twirling) to differing kinds of higher-level behaviors (such as lining of objects, echolalic speech, and preoccupation with spelling and numerical values). The children who changed to the highest levels of self-stimulatory behavior also showed the largest gains in treatment (as determined by IQ scores, school placement, etc.). The changes in self-stimulatory behaviors were attributed to the intense teaching of appropriate social behaviors and the explicit therapeutic suppression of low-level, self-stimulatory behaviors. The long-term therapeutic effects of changing from lower-to higher-level forms of self-stimulatory behavior were discussed.This study was supported by a grant from the National Institute of Mental Health (grant MH 11440). We want to express our thanks to the parents who allowed us to study their children. We gratefully acknowledge the assistance of Maria Audero with the design of the weighted measure, and Tracee Parker with the collection of the data. 相似文献
29.
Mark H. Lewis Alfred A. Baumeister Richard B. Mailman 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1987,20(3):253-258
A perceptual reinforcement theory of stereotyped movements is advanced by Lovaas, Newsom, and Hickman (1987) in an effort to integrate a number of diverse observations about the origins and maintenance of this behavior. We, in turn, argue that the theory, as presented, is logically flawed and fails to take into account important biological findings and theory concerning pathological stereotyped acts. An alternative theory, derived primarily from neurological concepts, is briefly described. 相似文献
30.