首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   27075篇
  免费   1090篇
  国内免费   11篇
  2020年   299篇
  2019年   358篇
  2018年   513篇
  2017年   529篇
  2016年   601篇
  2015年   451篇
  2014年   465篇
  2013年   2319篇
  2012年   868篇
  2011年   944篇
  2010年   555篇
  2009年   577篇
  2008年   713篇
  2007年   811篇
  2006年   674篇
  2005年   651篇
  2004年   632篇
  2003年   620篇
  2002年   625篇
  2001年   850篇
  2000年   806篇
  1999年   634篇
  1998年   316篇
  1997年   271篇
  1996年   288篇
  1995年   268篇
  1994年   262篇
  1992年   512篇
  1991年   479篇
  1990年   475篇
  1989年   430篇
  1988年   441篇
  1987年   412篇
  1986年   419篇
  1985年   424篇
  1984年   340篇
  1983年   325篇
  1982年   242篇
  1979年   388篇
  1978年   290篇
  1975年   288篇
  1974年   367篇
  1973年   375篇
  1972年   299篇
  1971年   290篇
  1970年   283篇
  1969年   255篇
  1968年   357篇
  1967年   310篇
  1966年   286篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
Developing readers often make anagrammatical errors (e.g. misreading pirates as parties), suggesting they use letter position flexibly during word recognition. However, while it is widely assumed that the occurrence of these errors decreases with increases in reading skill, empirical evidence to support this distinction is lacking. Accordingly, we compared the performance of developing child readers (aged 8–10 years) against the end‐state performance of skilled adult readers in a timed naming task, employing anagrams used previously in this area of research. Moreover, to explore the use of letter position by developing readers and skilled adult readers more fully, we used anagrams which, to form another word, required letter transpositions over only interior letter positions, or both interior and exterior letter positions. The patterns of effects across these two anagram types for the two groups of readers were very similar. In particular, both groups showed similarly slowed response times (and developing readers increased errors) for anagrams requiring only interior letter transpositions but not for anagrams that required exterior letter transpositions. This similarity in the naming performance of developing readers and skilled adult readers suggests that the end‐state skilled use of letter position is established earlier during reading development than is widely assumed.  相似文献   
53.
54.
55.
56.
This paper reports the operation of robust attentional bias to the top and right during perception of small, single geometric forms. Same/different judgements of successively presented standard and comparison forms are faster when local differences are located at top and right rather than in other regions of the forms. The bias persists when form size is reduced to approximately one degree of visual angle, and it is unaffected by saccadic eye movements and by instructions to attend to other reliably differentiating regions of the forms. Results lend support in various degrees to two of the possible explanations of the bias: (1) a static, skewed distribution of attentional resources around eye fixation; and (2) biased, covert scanning that commences invariably at the top and right of stim ulus forms. Origins of the bias in terms of possible left-hemispheric capacity for constructing representations of visual stimuli from parts, as well as in terms of reading experience and prevailing optic flow during locomotion through space are considered. Recent investigations of conditions under which the bias can be maintained or reduced are mentioned.  相似文献   
57.
58.
59.
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号