全文获取类型
收费全文 | 26417篇 |
免费 | 303篇 |
专业分类
26720篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 42篇 |
2022年 | 30篇 |
2021年 | 51篇 |
2020年 | 124篇 |
2019年 | 124篇 |
2018年 | 3607篇 |
2017年 | 2935篇 |
2016年 | 2420篇 |
2015年 | 346篇 |
2014年 | 221篇 |
2013年 | 757篇 |
2012年 | 839篇 |
2011年 | 2684篇 |
2010年 | 2671篇 |
2009年 | 1676篇 |
2008年 | 1926篇 |
2007年 | 2428篇 |
2006年 | 261篇 |
2005年 | 441篇 |
2004年 | 411篇 |
2003年 | 364篇 |
2002年 | 283篇 |
2001年 | 160篇 |
2000年 | 165篇 |
1999年 | 135篇 |
1998年 | 135篇 |
1997年 | 104篇 |
1996年 | 98篇 |
1995年 | 89篇 |
1994年 | 99篇 |
1993年 | 69篇 |
1992年 | 83篇 |
1991年 | 56篇 |
1990年 | 73篇 |
1989年 | 43篇 |
1988年 | 43篇 |
1987年 | 47篇 |
1986年 | 47篇 |
1985年 | 55篇 |
1984年 | 50篇 |
1983年 | 57篇 |
1982年 | 55篇 |
1981年 | 34篇 |
1980年 | 35篇 |
1979年 | 52篇 |
1978年 | 51篇 |
1977年 | 34篇 |
1976年 | 36篇 |
1974年 | 19篇 |
1972年 | 22篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
991.
In the present study, we examined the effects of selective response inhibition on motor production using response force measures
within a task that was based on that of Aron and Verbruggen (Psychological Science, 19, 1146–1153, 2008). In each trial, participants were signaled to respond bimanually with the two index fingers or the two
middle fingers. After a short delay, a stop signal was sometimes presented, indicating that one of the two finger responses
should be withheld. A given response was slowed when the response on the other hand was stopped, replicating a previously
observed stopping interference effect. In addition, the given response was also made more forcefully when the response on
the other hand was stopped, indicating that the requirement to stop one activated response has global motor-level consequences
for other responses that are to be carried out normally. 相似文献
992.
Landmark use has been demonstrated in a variety of organisms, yet the manner in which landmarks are encoded and subsequently
used appears to vary between and sometimes within species, even when faced with identical landmark arrays. In the present
experiments, orangutans and human children were shown a square array of identical landmarks and were trained to locate a hidden
goal in the centre of the array. In Experiments 1 and 2, the search space appeared to be discrete, with white gridlines dividing
up the space, and in Experiments 3a and 3b, the search space was uniformly coloured, making it appear continuous. In all experiments,
following training, subjects were given a single expansion test, to determine their landmark strategy use, based on peak search
activity. The orangutans appeared to use absolute directional vectors from individual landmarks, with peak search activities
on the inner corners of the square array, and they used this strategy persistently. In contrast, human children showed two
landmark-based strategies, absolute directional vectors and a relational or “middle” strategy, with the majority of children
starting their search in the middle region. Although some children, especially young children, persistently used one strategy
like the orangutans, many changed strategies when the original one failed to yield the hidden goal. 相似文献
993.
994.
995.
Gordon L. Flett Teresa Panico Paul L. Hewitt 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2011,30(2):105-116
The current study examined the associations among dimensions of perfectionism, Type A behavior, self-efficacy, distress, and
health symptoms in high school students. A sample of 73 high school students (34 boys, 39 girls) completed measures of self-oriented
perfectionism, socially prescribed perfectionism, Type A behavior, self-efficacy, depressive symptoms, and psychosomatic symptoms.
Correlational analyses found that self-oriented perfectionism was marginally related to Type A behavior. Students with elevated
levels of depressive symptoms were also characterized by self-oriented perfectionism, Type A behavior, low self-efficacy,
and health symptoms. In addition, health symptoms were linked with low self-efficacy. Simultaneous entry of several variables
into a regression analysis found that significant unique predictors of depression were low self-efficacy and elevated self-oriented
perfectionism. Similarly, a regression analysis found that low self-efficacy and high self-oriented perfectionism were unique
predictors of health symptoms. The findings highlight the distinctions between the perfectionism and the Type A constructs,
and support self-regulation models of depression and physical symptoms that include an emphasis on excessive perfectionistic
standards and low self-efficacy. We discuss the need for preventive interventions designed for perfectionistic adolescents
with low self-efficacy. 相似文献
996.
The main formal structures of generalized quantum theory are summarized. Recent progress has sharpened some of the concepts,
in particular the notion of an observable, the action of an observable on states (putting more emphasis on the role of proposition
observables), and the concept of generalized entanglement. Furthermore, the active role of the observer in the structure of
observables and the partitioning of systems is emphasized. 相似文献
997.
Athanasios Raftopoulos 《Synthese》2011,181(3):489-514
Macpherson (Nous 40(1):82–117, 2006) argues that the square/regular diamond figure threatens representationalism, construed
as the theory which holds that the phenomenal character is explained by the nonconceptual content of experience. Her argument
is the claim that representationalism is committed to the thesis that differences in the experience of ambiguous figures,
the gestalt switch, should be explained by differences in the NCC of perception of these figures. However, with respect to
the square/regular diamond and some other ambiguous figure representationalism fails to offer a unified account of how representational
content makes them ambiguous. In this paper, I aim, first, to offer a representationalist account of ambiguous figures and,
second, to examine and rebut Macpherson’s arguments. My main point is that in each ambiguous figure Macpherson discusses there
are differences in representational content that can explain differences in phenomenal character or content. The representational
differences are due to the ways the Cartesian frame of reference in which perceptual content is always cast cuts the figure,
underlying different properties of the figure with respect to the axes of the Cartesian frame of reference. 相似文献
998.
Stewart James Henry McCann 《Journal of Happiness Studies》2011,12(4):547-560
Relations between the emotional health domain of the Gallup-Healthways Well-Being Index and the Big Five personality factors
were determined at the American state level. State emotional health scores were based on the aggregated results of 353,039
phone interviews conducted throughout 2008 with a representative sample of US adults (Gallup 2009a). State z scores (Rentfrow et al. in Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 339–386, 2008) on neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness were based on 619,397 nationally representative
respondents to an internet survey between 1999 and 2005. State socioeconomic status (SES), urban percent, and white percent
based on 2000 and 2005 data served as demographic controls. Alaska and Hawaii were excluded. When the controls entered a hierarchical
multiple regression equation as a block and were followed by the Big Five selected stepwise, the controls accounted for 27.5%
of the emotional health variance and neuroticism accounted for another 35.3%. With stepwise selection of controls and then
Big Five variables, SES entered first (24.1%) and neuroticism entered second (32.4%). With stepwise selection from the combined
control and Big Five pool, neuroticism entered first (47.5%), SES entered second (9.1%), and urban percent entered third (4.9%).
Clearly, neuroticism and SES are the key contributors to emotional health variance and neuroticism makes the largest contribution.
States with higher proportions of neurotic individuals and lower SES tended to have populations with poorer emotional health.
Theoretical foundations for the link between neuroticism and emotional health at the state level and implications for changes
in state emotional health are discussed. 相似文献
999.
In a recent paper, Peter Singer suggests that some interesting new findings in experimental moral psychology support what
he has contended all along—namely that intuitions should play little or no role in adequate justifications of normative ethical
positions. Not only this but, according to Singer, these findings point to a central flaw in the method (or epistemological
theory) of reflective equilibrium used by many contemporary moral philosophers. In this paper, we try to defend reflective equilibrium from Singer’s attack
and, in part, we do this by discussing Singer’s own favoured moral methodology as outlined in his Practical Ethics. Although basing ethics solely on (certain kinds of) intuitions certainly is problematic, we argue, basing it solely on ‘reason’
gives rise to similar problems. The best solution would arguably be one which could strike a balance between the two—but,
we suggest, this is precisely what reflective equilibrium is all about. 相似文献
1000.
Owens KM Marvin ML Gelehrter TD Ruffin MT Uhlmann WR 《Journal of genetic counseling》2011,20(5):510-525
This study examined medical students’ and house officers’ opinions about the Surgeon General’s “My Family Health Portrait”
(MFHP) tool. Participants used the tool and were surveyed about tool mechanics, potential clinical uses, and barriers. None
of the 97 participants had previously used this tool. The average time to enter a family history was 15 min (range 3 to 45 min).
Participants agreed or strongly agreed that the MFHP tool is understandable (98%), easy to use (93%), and suitable for general
public use (84%). Sixty-seven percent would encourage their patients to use the tool; 39% would ensure staff assistance. Participants
would use the tool to identify patients at increased risk for disease (86%), record family history in the medical chart (84%),
recommend preventive health behaviors (80%), and refer to genetics services (72%). Concerns about use of the tool included
patient access, information accuracy, technical challenges, and the need for physician education on interpreting family history
information. 相似文献