全文获取类型
收费全文 | 26417篇 |
免费 | 303篇 |
专业分类
26720篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 42篇 |
2022年 | 30篇 |
2021年 | 51篇 |
2020年 | 124篇 |
2019年 | 124篇 |
2018年 | 3607篇 |
2017年 | 2935篇 |
2016年 | 2420篇 |
2015年 | 346篇 |
2014年 | 221篇 |
2013年 | 757篇 |
2012年 | 839篇 |
2011年 | 2684篇 |
2010年 | 2671篇 |
2009年 | 1676篇 |
2008年 | 1926篇 |
2007年 | 2428篇 |
2006年 | 261篇 |
2005年 | 441篇 |
2004年 | 411篇 |
2003年 | 364篇 |
2002年 | 283篇 |
2001年 | 160篇 |
2000年 | 165篇 |
1999年 | 135篇 |
1998年 | 135篇 |
1997年 | 104篇 |
1996年 | 98篇 |
1995年 | 89篇 |
1994年 | 99篇 |
1993年 | 69篇 |
1992年 | 83篇 |
1991年 | 56篇 |
1990年 | 73篇 |
1989年 | 43篇 |
1988年 | 43篇 |
1987年 | 47篇 |
1986年 | 47篇 |
1985年 | 55篇 |
1984年 | 50篇 |
1983年 | 57篇 |
1982年 | 55篇 |
1981年 | 34篇 |
1980年 | 35篇 |
1979年 | 52篇 |
1978年 | 51篇 |
1977年 | 34篇 |
1976年 | 36篇 |
1974年 | 19篇 |
1972年 | 22篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
92.
College counseling can best be understood as the intersection of a professional activity and an environment. The specialty has developed from the merging of several movements: vocational guidance, mental health, and student personnel work. These elements are reflected in the diversity of backgrounds and perspectives represented by college counselors and in differing models of counseling services. College counseling will be affected by trends including health care reform, scarcity of resources in higher education, changing student demographics, and complex societal issues. 相似文献
93.
This series of studies investigated the ability of literate adults to exploit communications options that are available to writers but have no counterpart in speech. Specifically it examined people's use of sketches when giving written directions to help a stranger cross town. When writing an informal letter to a friend most directions were given in prose paragraph style (Experiment 1). Almost no-one included a sketch, even if given a street map as an aid. Yet most subjects could draw adequate sketch maps (Experiment 2), and everyone said yes when asked whether they would include a sketch in a letter. So the previous choices were not constrained either by graphic skill or by judgements concerning the appropriateness of a sketch, When people were asked to 'design' the back page of a leaflet they still did not include a sketch (Experiment 3). Experiment 4 showed that any bias in the instructions was towards rather than against the use of sketches. The need to generate the sketch was removed in Experiment 5 where subjects composed the leaflet page from pre-formed sections that included alternative routes in both words and diagrams. Nearly all subjects now included a sketch in their directions. In case subjects previously, in haste, had not considered the possibility of including a sketch map, they were made aware of the drawing option by rating the usability of route information in a variety of communication styles immediately before writing their own directions (Experiment 6). People rated directions that included diagrams as significantly more usable, but still most subjects did not include a sketch in the directions they gave. So the communication styles that readers recognized as successful were not determining their choices as writers. It is suggested that, when giving written directions, inexperienced writers rely too heavily on how they would respond as speakers. 相似文献
94.
Frame multiplicity and policy fiascoes: Limits to explanation 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
The dynamics of intractability, frame multiplicity and intense controversy not only characterize policy formation struggles, as described by Schön and Rein, but also affect the evaluation of policies and government action. Policy analysts play an important part in the politics of policy evaluation. This article demonstrates that they tend to produce very different explanations for controversial, failing policies. Such differing explanations imply different causal attributions, different allocations of blame, and different lessons for future policy-making. Such differences are not random occurrences; they are embedded in the analytical frames that evaluators use to reconstruct policy processes and to explain their outcomes. It is shown that at least three deep-rooted frames can be reconstructed: fiascoes as failures of public problem solving, fiascoes as institutional imbalances, and fiascoes as system artefacts. One of the great epistemological challenges is how to deal with multiple analytical frames. The final part of the article discusses various strategies. 相似文献
95.
The purpose of the current study was to evaluate the school performance of residential youth who attended a public school setting. Direct comparisons were made between the residential youth and students in the community on a number of school measures. Results from a multivariate analysis of covariance indicated that the residential students had significantly lower grades, participated in significantly fewer extracurricular activities, and exhibited more problematic behaviors such as tardies and absences. These findings were consistent with previous research which found that many residential youth have a number of behavioral, social, and academic difficulties that interfere with school functioning. A number of recommendations were made regarding educational placement for residential youth. 相似文献
96.
Alan Kingstone Patricia A. Reuter-Lorenz Robert Fendrich C. Mark Wessinger 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1995,57(6):796-801
Extinguishing a fixation point shortly before, or concurrently with, the onset of a peripheral visual target reduces the latency of saccades to that target. Saslow (1967) hypothesized that thisgap effect might occur because fixation point offsets reduce the incidence of corrective microsaccades with an associated saccadic refractory period. In the present study, a robust gap effect was obtained. However, using a Purkinje image eyetracker with 1 arcmin of resolution, we found that fixation point offsets had no effect on the occurrence of microsaccades and that the occurrence of microsaccades had no impact on the magnitude of the gap effect. Microsaccades therefore do not appear to play any part in the production of the gap effect. 相似文献
97.
Mark A. Lumley Willfried Schramm Cynthia S. Pomerleau Ovide F. Pomerleau Richard H. Smith 《Behavior research methods》1995,27(4):470-475
Cortisol can be used to indicate stress level as well as to monitor certain disease states. Although cortisol can be sampled from blood and urine, saliva sampling has decided advantages. Unfortunately, whole saliva contains a number of substances that can metabolize or bind with cortisol, potentially confounding analysis and quantitation. We introduce a semipermeable pouch that accumulates a contaminant-free salivary ultrafiltrate, thereby overcoming a number of the problems encountered with whole saliva. In three studies, we demonstrate accuracy and utility of the device for cortisol determination: (1) in an artificial medium, 60%–77% of the cortisol was recovered in the ultrafiltrate; (2) in vivo, ultrafiltrate cortisol correlated highly with whole-saliva cortisol collected under ideal conditions; and (3) ultrafiltrate cortisol evinced positive relationships with depression and cigarette use, consistent with studies in the literature. We conclude that this device and saliva-filtering technologies in general are useful in applications requiring quantitation of cortisol. 相似文献
98.
Robert H. Gilkey Michael D. Good Mark A. Ericson John Brinkman John M. Stewart 《Behavior research methods》1995,27(1):1-11
A technique is described for rapidly collecting responses in auditory-localization experiments. Subjects indicate the perceived direction of the sound by pointing at a 20-cm-diam spherical model. In Experiment 1, the subjects judged the direction of a broadband signal, which could originate from any of 239 directions ranging through 360° of azimuth and between ?45° and +90° of elevation. Using this technique, the subjects responded 2–8 times more rapidly than previous subjects who used either a verbal-reporting technique or a head-pointing technique. Localization judgments were as accurate as they had been with verbal reports, but were not as accurate as judgments collected using the head-pointing technique. In Experiment 2, the signal was turned off and the experimenter read the spherical coordinates of the signal location to the subjects. The subjects pointed to these coordinates more accurately than they had judged the direction of the sounds in Experiment 1, suggesting that the response technique had not been the limiting factor in that experiment. Circumstances relevant to the choice of response techniques for auditory-localization experiments are discussed. 相似文献
99.
100.
In contrast to previous research on apologies, which has examined their role in enhancing impressions of those issuing them
(e.g., Darby and Schlenker, 1989), the research reported here draws upon Goffman’s (1955) analysis of the “corrective cycle”
and is concerned with constraints associated with the receipt of apologies. Study 1 examines the implications for the actor
of accepting, not accepting, or rejecting apologies. It is demonstrated that across a variety of judgments, most positive
views of the actor result when apologies are accepted; least positive views are associated with their rejection. A follow-up
study sought to establish whether this effect would occur under circumstances in which unconvincing apologies are rejected.
Results indicate that the apology’s status (convincing versus unconvincing) has no bearing on perceptions of actors. Based
on a hypothetical role-play format, Study 2 addresses the matter of whether actors experience a subjective sense of constraint
upon receipt of unsatisfactory apologies. The data suggest that there exists a pronounced tendency to accept such apologies,
though typically with conditions that would be specified to the offender. More general implications of the data are discussed. 相似文献