全文获取类型
收费全文 | 41032篇 |
免费 | 423篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
41458篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 277篇 |
2019年 | 285篇 |
2018年 | 3764篇 |
2017年 | 3090篇 |
2016年 | 2613篇 |
2015年 | 530篇 |
2014年 | 456篇 |
2013年 | 1750篇 |
2012年 | 1265篇 |
2011年 | 3030篇 |
2010年 | 2910篇 |
2009年 | 1896篇 |
2008年 | 2271篇 |
2007年 | 2763篇 |
2006年 | 599篇 |
2005年 | 764篇 |
2004年 | 687篇 |
2003年 | 593篇 |
2002年 | 547篇 |
2001年 | 805篇 |
2000年 | 808篇 |
1999年 | 565篇 |
1998年 | 290篇 |
1997年 | 258篇 |
1996年 | 245篇 |
1995年 | 212篇 |
1994年 | 217篇 |
1993年 | 175篇 |
1992年 | 430篇 |
1991年 | 381篇 |
1990年 | 412篇 |
1989年 | 332篇 |
1988年 | 311篇 |
1987年 | 319篇 |
1986年 | 292篇 |
1985年 | 295篇 |
1984年 | 276篇 |
1983年 | 251篇 |
1982年 | 199篇 |
1979年 | 259篇 |
1978年 | 216篇 |
1977年 | 174篇 |
1975年 | 203篇 |
1974年 | 245篇 |
1973年 | 246篇 |
1972年 | 195篇 |
1971年 | 189篇 |
1969年 | 181篇 |
1968年 | 219篇 |
1967年 | 196篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
231.
Recent memory theory has emphasized the concept ofneed probability—that is, the probability that a given piece of learned information will be tested at some point in the future. It has been proposed that, in real-world situations, need probability declines over time and that the memory-loss rate is calibrated to match the progressive reduction in need probability (J. R. Anderson & Schooler, 1991). The present experiments were designed to examine the influence of the slope of the need-probability curve on the slope of the retention curve. On each of several trials, subjects memorized a list of digits, then retained the digits in memory for 1, 2, 4, 8, or 16 sec. Some trials ended with a recall test; other trials ended with the message, “no test.” In Experiment 1, the likelihood of encountering a memory test (i.e., the need probability) was made to either increase or decrease as the retention interval increased; in Experiment 2, need probability either was flat (invariant across retention intervals) or decreased as the retention interval increased. The results indicated that the shape of the need-probability curve influenced the slope of the retention curve (Experiment 1) and that the effect became larger as the experimental session progressed (Experiment 2). The findings support the notion that memory adapts to need probabilities and that the rate of forgetting is influenced by the slope of the need-probability curve. In addition, all of the forgetting curves approximated a power function, suggesting that need probability influences the slope but not the form of forgetting. 相似文献
232.
J Hidalgo E Belloso J Hernandez T Gasull A Molinero 《Stress (Amsterdam, Netherlands)》1997,1(4):231-240
The metallothionein (MT) gene family consists of four members (MT-I through -IV) that are tightly regulated during development. Whereas MT-I and MT-II are widely expressed isoforms, MT-III has been found to be mainly expressed in the central nervous system in adult animals, and is the only isoform that inhibits survival and neurite formation of cortical neurons in vitro. A number of models of brain injury have been shown to affect MT-III mRNA levels, which has been suggested to be related to the putative neurotrophic role of this protein. However, a stress response will presumably be associated to the brain injury which could, in turn, drive MT-III regulation. In the present report the effect of a classical stress model, immobilization stress, on brain MT regulation has been studied in rats. MT-I+II protein levels were measured by radioimmunoassay in up to eight brain areas and, as expected, it was found that stress increased selectively MT-I+II levels. Adrenalectomy (ADX) had a general decreasing effect on basal MT-I+II levels; however, ADX blunted the MT-I+II response to stress in cerebellum and presumably in frontal cortex and medulla plus pons but not in the hypothalamus. MT-I mRNA measurements were in accordance with the MT-I+II protein levels in the brain areas studied. In contrast to MT-I mRNA, MT-III mRNA levels of brain cortex tended to decrease during stress, although this effect was not statistically significant. ADX also tended to decrease basal MT-III mRNA levels. Northern blot assays of pooled mRNAs suggested similar differential regulation of these two brain MT isoforms in the cerebellum. These results indicate that glucocorticoids mediate brain MT-I+II response to stress in some but not all brain areas, that a role of these hormones is likely also for MT-III, and that the regulation of MT isoforms differs substantially in the brain. 相似文献
233.
This article demonstrates that there is a role for behavior-analytic techniques in the area of farm animal welfare and provides examples of the kinds of work that can be done. Behavior-analytic procedures, specifically those used in the study of psychophysics, preference, and demand, can provide answers to three questions people concerned with the welfare of farm animals are likely to ask: What can the animals detect? What do they like and dislike? What will they work to attain or preserve? Such information certainly is necessary for making reasonable decisions about animal welfare, although it is not sufficient in and of itself. 相似文献
234.
235.
CLUSTERS PRECEDE SHAPES IN PERCEPTUAL ORGANIZATION 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Abstract —Does perceptual grouping require attention? Recent controversy on this question may be caused by a conflation of two aspects of grouping: element clustering (determining which elements belong together) and shape formation (determining cluster boundaries). In Experiment 1, observers enumerated diamonds that were drawn with either lines or dots. These two types of stimuli were subitized (enumerated rapidly and accurately in the range from one to three items) equally well, suggesting that clustering dots into countable entities did not detnand attention. In contrast, when target diamonds were enumerated among distractor squares in Experiment 2. only line-drawn items could be subitized. We propose that clustering and shape formation not only involve different perceptual processes, but play different functional roles in vision. 相似文献
236.
T.D. Borkovec 《Psychological science》1997,8(3):145-147
Abstract— This article argues that the greatest progress in developing more effective psychotherapist will come from a renewed emphasis on designing and conducting therapy research as basic science devoted lo the acquisition of cause-and-effect relationships and from collaborations between clinical researchers and basic researchers from other domains of psychology. 相似文献
237.
238.
Jörg R. J. Schirra 《Kognitionswissenschaft》1997,6(4):177-195
How can we explain that an assertion on something perceived can be understood in the same manner by somebody who cannot perceive that scene? This problem bases the interest in computational linguistics in how listener modeling could possibly be harmonized with reference semantics. Mental images substituting real perception appear as a way out. The architecture of the listener model has to be adapted to the creation and use of such pictorial data structures. Furthermore, the relation between the latter and a verbal (i. e., propositional) representation must be understood. The resulting architecture of a listener model with reference semantics can be employed to solve communicational problems from three general classes in a better way, as is demonstrated by an example implementation. 相似文献
239.
The American Journal of Psychoanalysis - 相似文献
240.
Rosana Glat Karen Gould Lawrence T. Stoddard Murray Sidman 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1994,27(4):699-704
This case study describes initially unsuccessful attempts to use the delayed-cue procedure to teach conditional discriminations to an individual with moderate mental retardation. The task was matching printed-word comparison stimuli to dictated-name sample stimuli. In three experiments, the subject typically waited for the delayed cue unless differential responses to the dictated samples (repeating the sample names) were required. Hence, the study provides an example of a way to make the delayed-cue method more effective. The stimulus control bases for the results are discussed. 相似文献