全文获取类型
收费全文 | 25534篇 |
免费 | 248篇 |
专业分类
25782篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 41篇 |
2022年 | 28篇 |
2021年 | 46篇 |
2020年 | 110篇 |
2019年 | 110篇 |
2018年 | 3588篇 |
2017年 | 2916篇 |
2016年 | 2399篇 |
2015年 | 323篇 |
2014年 | 191篇 |
2013年 | 663篇 |
2012年 | 800篇 |
2011年 | 2646篇 |
2010年 | 2654篇 |
2009年 | 1650篇 |
2008年 | 1905篇 |
2007年 | 2398篇 |
2006年 | 240篇 |
2005年 | 421篇 |
2004年 | 386篇 |
2003年 | 335篇 |
2002年 | 256篇 |
2001年 | 137篇 |
2000年 | 147篇 |
1999年 | 115篇 |
1998年 | 124篇 |
1997年 | 95篇 |
1996年 | 86篇 |
1995年 | 76篇 |
1994年 | 87篇 |
1993年 | 56篇 |
1992年 | 71篇 |
1991年 | 42篇 |
1990年 | 58篇 |
1989年 | 31篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 38篇 |
1986年 | 35篇 |
1985年 | 37篇 |
1984年 | 34篇 |
1983年 | 51篇 |
1982年 | 41篇 |
1981年 | 32篇 |
1980年 | 29篇 |
1979年 | 39篇 |
1978年 | 44篇 |
1977年 | 27篇 |
1976年 | 29篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
Gloria S. Waters Margaret Bruck Mark Seidenberg 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1985,39(3):511-530
The use of spelling-sound information in both reading and spelling was evaluated by having children read and spell nonwords and five types of words that differed in terms of their regularity for reading and spelling. The subjects were grade 3 children who had been psychometrically defined as good readers and good spellers (“good”), good readers and poor spellers (“mixed”), or poor readers and poor spellers (“poor”). Results indicated that all children attempted to use spelling-sound correspondences in both reading and spelling, although children in both the mixed and the poor groups had weaker knowledge of these correspondences and were less systematic in their use of them. Furthermore, even though the children in the mixed group had been matched with children in the good group on reading comprehension, the number and type of errors made by the mixed subjects on both the reading and spelling tasks were more similar to those of the poor subjects than to those of the good subjects. 相似文献
32.
Mark Bennett 《Current Psychology》1985,4(4):323-329
It was hypothesized (1) that with age children would rely increasingly upon information about intention, rather than overt
behavior, when attributing dispositional features to others; and (2) that, developmentally, “restricted” dispositional attributions
(i.e., those that assume the operation of highly specific situation-response relationships) would precede “generalized” dispositional
attributions (which assume the operation of a dispositional concept between broad classes of situations and responses). An
experiment is reported that provides support for the first, though not the second, of these hypotheses. 相似文献
33.
Mark H. Lewis Alfred A. Baumeister Richard B. Mailman 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1987,20(3):253-258
A perceptual reinforcement theory of stereotyped movements is advanced by Lovaas, Newsom, and Hickman (1987) in an effort to integrate a number of diverse observations about the origins and maintenance of this behavior. We, in turn, argue that the theory, as presented, is logically flawed and fails to take into account important biological findings and theory concerning pathological stereotyped acts. An alternative theory, derived primarily from neurological concepts, is briefly described. 相似文献
34.
The Psychological Record - 相似文献
35.
Strengthening local alcohol programs so as to emphasize primary prevention is an active concern among mental health professionals; yet interventions intended to achieve this aim have produced only modest shifts in program orientation in most communities. An analysis of data obtained from those active in local affairs in a small community, using Q-sort techniques, suggested that contrasting perspectives toward alcohol abuse as a social problem may stand as an unseen barrier to relevant change. 相似文献
36.
Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: a study of sitting and stair climbing 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Previous work has shown that both the perceived and actual critical (maximum) heights of surfaces that afford "sitting on " and "climbing on" can be expressed as constant proportions of each actor's leg length. The current study provides evidence that these judgments of critical action boundaries are based on an existing source of size and distance information that is already scaled with reference to the actor's eyeheight. In Experiment 1 changes in judgments of "perceived eyeheight" (an index of the intrinsic scalar) as a function of viewing distance were shown to be highly correlated with changes in the maximum height that was perceived to afford sitting on or climbing on. In Experiments 2 and 3 observers wore 10-cm blocks and made judgments about whether the heights of various surfaces afforded sitting or climbing. The use of eyeheight-scaled information as the basis for their estimates predicted the obtained pattern of errors in these judgments. With a modicum of experience wearing the blocks, however, observers were able to retune accurately their critical action boundary to a degree that would not have been predicted from their consistent overestimation of the height of the block on which they were standing. These results have implications for understanding how observers obtain information about their specific action boundary. 相似文献
37.
Previous research has failed to demonstrate unambiguously the relative stability across time and consistency across tasks of individual differences in categorization, or categorization breadth The present study of categorizing behavior in college students assesses breadth at each of four points in time over a six-week period Results are consistent with an interpretation of breadth as a highly stable individual difference variable, but one that is constrained by the nature of the stimulus sets employed High stability coefficients were observed for each of two redundant tasks In contrast, cross-task consistency indices were generally low Personality correlates of breadth were similarly weak and inconsistent Several variables are suggested that may attenuate the generality of categorization breadth The existence of multiple styles of categorization is discussed 相似文献
38.
Mark I. Appelbaum 《Psychometrika》1986,51(1):53-56
Approximately 15% of all papers appearing inPsychometrika during the period 1960–1984 have been statistical or data analytic in nature. While these papers cover a very wide range of issues and are technically quite similar to those appearing in other major statistical journals, collectively they possess features which make them identifiablyPsychometrika. 相似文献
39.
Frank D. Lewis William F. Blackerby Janet Rainwater Ross Mark L. Guth Ronald F. Cronkey Melvin J. White Tina Cook 《Behavioral Interventions》1986,1(4):265-274
This study evaluated the relative efficacy of long (1 minute) and short (15 seconds) durations of negative practice for reducing self-stimulatory leg pounding of a traumatically brain-injured adult. In individual social skill training classes, long negative practice and short negative practice were administered concurrently in an alternating treatments design. Generalization of treatment effects outside of training sessions was assessed during the client's lunch period. The results revealed that both durations of negative practice were equally effective in reducing leg pounding during training sessions. However, the effects of training did not generalize to settings in which the practice contingency was not enforced. Pounding was more frequent and negative practice somewhat less effective during periods of high social stimulation. 相似文献
40.
A Hypermasculinity Inventory was developed to measure a macho personality constellation consisting of three components: (a) calloused sex attitudes toward women, (b) violence as manly, and (c) danger as exciting. The 30 forced-choice items were selected by a two-stage internal consistency item analysis. Issues of substantive and structural validity were addressed by considering item content, test format, homogeneity of items, and the factor structure of items. The Cronbach α coefficient for the Hypermasculinity Inventory was .89 in the present sample of 135 college men. External validity was assessed by correlating scores of the Hypermasculinity Inventory with self-reported drug use, r(135) = .26, p < .01, aggressive behavior r(135) = .65, p < .001, and dangerous driving, r(136) = .47, p < .001, following alcohol consumption, and delinquent behavior during the high school years, r(135) = .38, p < .01. Construct validity was supported further by a pattern of theoretically meaningful correlations with the Personality Research Form (D. N. Jackson, 1974, Goshen, NY: Research Psychologists Press). The discussion considered further research that is needed to adduce additional evidence for the construct validity of the Hypermasculinity Inventory as a measure of the macho personality pattern. 相似文献