首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   27497篇
  免费   325篇
  2023年   42篇
  2022年   32篇
  2021年   60篇
  2020年   138篇
  2019年   141篇
  2018年   3627篇
  2017年   2957篇
  2016年   2436篇
  2015年   366篇
  2014年   238篇
  2013年   861篇
  2012年   882篇
  2011年   2715篇
  2010年   2687篇
  2009年   1692篇
  2008年   1963篇
  2007年   2452篇
  2006年   326篇
  2005年   480篇
  2004年   443篇
  2003年   397篇
  2002年   317篇
  2001年   170篇
  2000年   183篇
  1999年   160篇
  1998年   159篇
  1997年   129篇
  1996年   130篇
  1995年   104篇
  1994年   118篇
  1993年   90篇
  1992年   92篇
  1991年   65篇
  1990年   86篇
  1989年   54篇
  1988年   47篇
  1987年   67篇
  1986年   58篇
  1985年   62篇
  1984年   58篇
  1983年   78篇
  1982年   81篇
  1981年   61篇
  1980年   56篇
  1979年   71篇
  1978年   67篇
  1977年   51篇
  1976年   55篇
  1975年   35篇
  1974年   29篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 234 毫秒
161.
Johnson  Barbara E.  Kuck  Douglas L.  Schander  Patricia R. 《Sex roles》1997,36(11-12):693-707
Many myths have been identified surrounding rape, rapists, and rape victims. This study reexamines the acceptance of rape myths across gender role ideologies and selected demographic characteristics to identify core myths. Three myth categories were established and investigated: blaming the woman, excusing the man, and justifications for acquaintance rape. Findings indicate that rape myths remain prevalent and adherence to myths is related to demographic factors and gender role attitudes. Overall, respondents tend to excuse the man more than blame the woman. Males accept rape myths more than females. Racial differences emerged most strongly on the justifications for acquaintance rape dimension. Individuals with a conservative gender role ideology believe rape myths more than those with more liberal ideologies. While core myths did not emerge from the data, the most revealing finding is that summative scaling techniques used in previous studies may mask important differences, between and within the three dimensions, in rape myth acceptance among the groups studied.  相似文献   
162.
163.
164.
165.
How can we explain that an assertion on something perceived can be understood in the same manner by somebody who cannot perceive that scene? This problem bases the interest in computational linguistics in how listener modeling could possibly be harmonized with reference semantics. Mental images substituting real perception appear as a way out. The architecture of the listener model has to be adapted to the creation and use of such pictorial data structures. Furthermore, the relation between the latter and a verbal (i. e., propositional) representation must be understood. The resulting architecture of a listener model with reference semantics can be employed to solve communicational problems from three general classes in a better way, as is demonstrated by an example implementation.  相似文献   
166.
167.
The American Journal of Psychoanalysis -  相似文献   
168.
169.
Traditional studies of deterrence have focused on punishment with less regard for the rewards of both crime and noncrime. Influenced by work in economics and behavioral psychology, more recent studies have departed from tradition by incorporating rewards for crime. To this extent, they should be regarded as advances over the more traditional approaches. Notwithstanding these advances, variations in both the probability and magnitude of reward for noncrime have not been systematically included in these more recent theories of choice. In an attempt to determine whether opportunities for noncrime are either central or trivial to the criminal decision-making process, the present study fitted two alternative models to experimental data involving risk-taking: (a) the economic utility model employed by Piliavin, Gartner, Thornton, and Matsueda (1986) in their study of criminal choice; and (b) the satisfaction balance model developed by Gray and Tallman (1984). Results showed that while both models explained significant amounts of variation in the dependent variable, the Gray-Tallman model provided a substantially better fit of the data. Despite limitations inherent in experimental studies as, for example, limitations surrounding the issue of external validity, the findings strongly suggest that opportunities for noncrime are as important as rewards and costs for crime in the process by which criminal decisions are made.  相似文献   
170.
The Heritability of Characteristics Associated with Dispositional Empathy   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
ABSTRACT This investigation used data from Loehlin and Nichols's (1976) study of over 800 sets of twins to examine evidence for the heritability of three facets of empathy: empathic concern, personal distress, and perspective taking. Expert judges first identified sets of adjectives, included within Loehlin and Nichols's original data, which reflected each empathy construct; these items were then validated in an independent sample. Comparisons of the responses given to these items by identical and fraternal twins in the Loehlin and Nichols investigation revealed evidence of significant heritability for characteristics associated with the two affective facets of empathy—empathic concern and personal distress—but not for the nonaffective construct of perspective taking. This pattern is consistent with the view that temperamental emotionality may underlie the heritability of affective empathy.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号