全文获取类型
收费全文 | 20937篇 |
免费 | 31篇 |
专业分类
20968篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 14篇 |
2018年 | 3492篇 |
2017年 | 2819篇 |
2016年 | 2256篇 |
2015年 | 200篇 |
2014年 | 88篇 |
2013年 | 122篇 |
2012年 | 579篇 |
2011年 | 2398篇 |
2010年 | 2528篇 |
2009年 | 1482篇 |
2008年 | 1710篇 |
2007年 | 2184篇 |
2006年 | 34篇 |
2005年 | 225篇 |
2004年 | 188篇 |
2003年 | 133篇 |
2002年 | 79篇 |
2001年 | 51篇 |
2000年 | 63篇 |
1999年 | 25篇 |
1998年 | 31篇 |
1997年 | 27篇 |
1996年 | 12篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 16篇 |
1989年 | 8篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1987年 | 11篇 |
1986年 | 7篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 7篇 |
1970年 | 8篇 |
1968年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
32.
Dennis M. Maloney Terry M. Harper Curtis J. Braukmann Dean L. Fixsen Elery L. Phillips Montrose M. Wolf 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1976,9(3):371-371
The use of conversation-related skills by youthful offenders can influence social interactions with adults. These behaviors are also likely to be useful to adolescents after their release from a treatment program (Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 1972, 5 , 343–372). Four girls, aged 13 to 15 yr, residing at Achievement Place for Girls in Lawrence, Kansas, received training on conversation-related behaviors. A multiple-baseline design across youths and across behaviors was used. Youth answer-volunteering in response to questions and three youth nonverbal components (“hand on face”, “hand at rest”, and “facial orientation”) were measured during daily 10-min sessions with a simulated guest in the group home's living room. Answer-volunteering was scored each session as the per cent of 13 “secondary” questions that the simulated guest did not have to ask following 10 “primary” questions. The three nonverbal components were scored according to their occurrence during 10-sec intervals and the resultant scores were averaged per session for an overall appropriate nonverbal score. The girls individually earned points within the home's token economy for participating in each session and additional points were awarded after training if preselected behavioral criteria were achieved for each of the two behavior categories per girl. Some of the training sessions were led by a “teaching-parent” (specially trained houseparent) while others were led by individual girls. Point consequences were administered by both the teaching-parent and by the “peer-trainers”. The average observed rate of answer-volunteering by the girls during pretraining sessions was 30% for S1, 30% for S2, 23% for S3, and 68% for S4. The average rate of answer-volunteering during posttraining sessions was: S1 = 92%, S2 = 89%, S3 = 90%, and S4 = 98%. The average nonverbal score during pretraining sessions was 82% for S1, 53% for S2, 60% for S3, and 82% for S4. The average nonverbal score during posttraining sessions was: S1 = 98%, S2 = 98%, S3 = 98%, and S4 = 100%. Videotapes of the sessions were shown in a random sequence to four adults (probation officer, social worker, etc who represented “significant others” for the youths' future success in the community. The adults judged posttraining tapes on the average as more appropriate 100% of the time for S1, 100% of the time for S2, 90% of the time for S3, and 70% of the time for S4. The study demonstrated that training of conversation-related skills is feasible with predelinquent girls, that the girls can help train each other, and that social validation of the training results is possible. 相似文献
33.
Confirmatory factor analyses of the 15 cognitive abilities tests from the Hawaii Family Study of Cognition were conducted on between-family (BF) means and within-family (WF) differences for 370 AEA (Americans of European ancestry) and 116 AJA (Americans of Japanese ancestry) sibling pairs. Difference chi-square significance tests, goodness-of-fit indices, and congruence coefficients for the estimated loadings on four specific cognitive abilities factors and on the second-order general intelligence factor nearly all indicated that the between-family factor structures were not significantly different from the within-family structures for both AEA and AJA siblings (the AEA and AJA structures were also not significantly different). The similarity of the BF and WF structures suggests that the genetic and environmental influences underlying cognitive abilities are “intrinsic” in nature, that is, not just due to between-family differences in culture, status, values, and fortuitous cross-assortative mating. 相似文献
34.
35.
36.
37.
38.
39.
The results of two experiments indicate that decision-makers separately distort their interpretations of decision criteria and evaluations of information in a manner that justifies their decisions. In Study 1, participants reported a yes/no decision either before or after they rated the applicable decision criterion and problem information. Participants distorted their criterion and information ratings more when they reported their decisions before, rather than after, they rated the decision criterion and problem information. Study 2 demonstrated that distortion of decision criterion ratings occurred after a decision had been made whereas distortion of information evaluations occurred during the process of reaching a decision. These results are consistent with a two-stage model of decision-making in which the distortion of decision criteria is distinct from the distortion of decision information. 相似文献
40.
Warren R. Stanton 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2002,21(2):176-198
Stage theories are commonly used to represent the structure of cognitive processes in the domains of cognitive processing
and cognitive development. Hierarchical unidimensional theories consisting of a single sequence of stages through which development
proceeds in one direction only have predominated. However, a review of stage theories revealed that a two-dimensional, interactive
(multidimensional) structure is warranted. Particularly in the area of cognitive development, results of research have supported
subdivision of the stages of prominent unidimensional theories. Following this trend a small number of theorists have developed
multidimensional theories, most of which consist of a sequence of stages in which each stage contains a number of subsidiary
stages or levels. The emergence of a similar trend in other domains is examined, and the prospects of using an existing multidimensional
framework to enable future comparisons and integration of different stage theories is explored. 相似文献