首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   433篇
  免费   22篇
  2022年   3篇
  2021年   4篇
  2020年   8篇
  2019年   10篇
  2018年   12篇
  2017年   15篇
  2016年   14篇
  2015年   11篇
  2014年   12篇
  2013年   41篇
  2012年   15篇
  2011年   17篇
  2010年   11篇
  2009年   12篇
  2008年   16篇
  2007年   25篇
  2006年   19篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   9篇
  2003年   15篇
  2002年   8篇
  2001年   10篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   7篇
  1998年   5篇
  1997年   4篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   3篇
  1993年   3篇
  1992年   4篇
  1991年   5篇
  1989年   5篇
  1988年   4篇
  1979年   4篇
  1977年   3篇
  1975年   8篇
  1974年   7篇
  1973年   4篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   3篇
  1970年   5篇
  1969年   3篇
  1968年   4篇
  1965年   2篇
  1938年   4篇
  1937年   2篇
  1935年   8篇
  1934年   2篇
  1931年   2篇
  1930年   2篇
排序方式: 共有455条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
181.
    
Otto Neurath 《Erkenntnis》1934,4(1):346-362
Ohne Zusammenfassung Erwiderung auf Moritz Schlick „über das Fundament der Erkenntnis”, Erkenntnis IV, S. 79, und Thilo Vogel „Bemerkungen zur Ausfagentheorie des radikalen Phyfikalismus”, Erkenntnis IV, S.160. Bezug wird genommen auf Fred Bon, „Der Gegenftand der Pfychologie”, Erkenntnis IV, S. 363. Der im folgenden vertretene Standpunkt ift ausführlicher entwickelt in Otto Neurath, „Phyfikalismus”, Scientia 1931, „Soziologie im Phyfikalismus” (insbefondere: I. Metaphyfikfreier Phyfikalismus, 2. Einheitsfprache des Phyfikalismus), Erkenntnis II, 1931, „Protokollf?tze”, Erkenntnis III, 1932, „Einheitswiffenfchaft und Pfychologie” in Schriftenreihe Einheitswiffenfchaft, Heft I, Gerold & Co., Wien 1933. Au?erdem vergleiche man Philipp Frank „Das Kaufalgefetz und feine Grenzen” in Schriften zur Wiffenfchaftlichen Weltauffaffung, Bd. IV, Julius Springer, Wien 1932, ebenfo die letzten Ver?ffentlichungen Rudolf Carnaps; eine kurze Zufammenfaffung davon in: „Die Aufgaben der Wiffenfchaftslogik” in Schriftenreihe Einheitswiffenfchaft, Heft III, Gerold & Co., Wien 1934.  相似文献   
182.
183.
Otto Rank (1884–1939) wrote the present work at the height of his creative powers, betweenWill Therapy andArt and Artist. Here he presents a sweeping history of psychology—individual and social—from the animistic era to psychoanalysis. An earlier translation (by William D. Turner, 1950) was incomplete and somewhat inaccurate. Unlike Sigmund Freud, his mentor, Rank viewed religion with respect and clarifies its role in individual and communal life through this study of soul-belief through the ages. The book contains important insights on immortality, will, dreams, Judaism and Christianity, Hamlet and Don Juan, Jung and Adler, and Freud himself. (Translated by Gregory C. Richter and E. James Lieberman) Ph.D., is Associate Professor in the Division of Language and Literature, Truman State University, Kirksville, Missouri. He translated Otto Rank'sThe Incest Theme in Literature and Legend (1912), Johns Hopkins University Press, 1991. E. James Lieberman, M.D., author ofActs of Will: The Life and Work of Otto Rank (1985), is Clinical Professor of Psychiatry, George Washington University School of Medicine. This excert from the book of the same title by Otto Rank is printed with the permission of the publisher, The Johns Hopkins University Press, for which we are grateful.  相似文献   
184.
185.
We investigated how both objective and subjective organizations affect perceptual organization and how this perceptual organization, in turn, influences observers’ performance in a localization search task. Two groups of observers viewing exactly the same stimuli (objective organization) performed in significantly different ways, depending on how they were induced to parse the display (subjective organization). In Experiments 1 and 2, the observers were asked to describe the location of a tilted target among a varying number of vertical or horizontal distractors. Subjective organization was induced by instructing observers to parse the display into either three horizontal regions (rows) or three vertical regions (columns). The position of the target was critical: location performance, as assessed by reaction time and errors, was consistently impaired at the locations adjacent to the boundaries defining the regions, producing what we refer to as thesubjective boundary effect. Furthermore, the extent of this effect depended on whether the stimulus-driven and conceptually driven information concurred or conflicted. This made location information more or less accessible. In Experiment 1, the strength of objective grouping was a function of the proximity of the items (near or far conditions) and their orientation in a 6×6 matrix. In Experiment 2, the strength of objective grouping was a function of similarity of color (items were color coded by rows or by columns) and the orientation of the items in a 9×9 matrix. The subjective boundary effect was more pronounced when the display promoted grouping in the direction orthogonal to that of the task (e.g., when observers parsed by rows but vertical distractors were closer together [Experiment 1] or color coded [Experiment 2] to induce global columns). In contrast, this effect decreased when the direction of both objective and subjective organizations was parallel (e.g., when observers parsed by rows and horizontal distractors were closer together [Experiment 1] or were color coded [Experiment 2] to induce global rows). A localization search task proved to be an ideal forum in which objective and subjective organizations interacted. We discuss how these results indicated that observers’ performance in a localization task was determined by the interaction of objective and subjective organizations, and that the resulting perceptual organization constrained coarse location information.  相似文献   
186.
187.
The role of mental health professionals testifying as expert witnesses has been the subject of increasing criticism. Cases in which opposing experts reach different conclusions are dismissed as “battles of the experts” and psychologists and psychiatrists are described as “hired guns.” This preliminary, analogue study examined the degree to which the opinions and testimony of mental health professionals may differ according to which side retains the expert. Results provided some support for the proposition that mental health professionals' testimony may vary according to the side by which they are retained.  相似文献   
188.
Sex differences and personality factors in responsivity to pain   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
This study investigated the relationship between measured levels of masculinity-femininity, social desirability, and responsivity to pain in men and women. The literature suggests that the two personality variables might provide an explanation for the common findings of higher pain thresholds and tolerances in men than in women. A significant interaction was found between masculinity-femininity and sex for pain thresholds. Analysis of this interaction indicated that for men, but not women, there was a significant correlation between masculinity-femininity and pain, where higher masculinity was associated with higher pain thresholds. However, this finding did not account for the sex difference in pain threshold. The sex of the subject remained a significant predictor of both pain thresholds and tolerances after allowing for the influence of masculinity-femininity, social desirability, and their associated interactions.  相似文献   
189.
190.
Taking Bandura's Social Cognitive Theory as our starting point, we tested how efficacy beliefs (self‐efficacy and perceived collective efficacy) reciprocally influence activity engagement (vigor, dedication, and absorption) indirectly through their impact on positive affect (enthusiasm, satisfaction, and comfort) over time. We conducted two longitudinal studies using independent samples. Study 1 is a two‐wave longitudinal field study that examines gain cycles regarding the dynamic relationships among self‐efficacy, positive affect, and work engagement in 274 secondary school teachers. Study 2 is a three‐wave longitudinal laboratory study about gain spirals in the dynamic relationships among collective efficacy beliefs, positive affect, and task engagement in 100 university students working in groups. Our findings show that: (1) efficacy beliefs reciprocally influence activity engagement indirectly through their impact on positive affect over time; (2) enthusiasm is the positive affect with the strongest effect on activity engagement; and (3) a gain spiral exists whereby efficacy beliefs increase over time due to engagement and positive affect (most notably enthusiasm). Finally, we discuss the theoretical and practical implications in terms of Social Cognitive Theory.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号