全文获取类型
收费全文 | 353篇 |
免费 | 15篇 |
专业分类
368篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 9篇 |
2018年 | 12篇 |
2017年 | 13篇 |
2016年 | 20篇 |
2015年 | 18篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 40篇 |
2012年 | 18篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 12篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 18篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 16篇 |
2004年 | 5篇 |
2003年 | 7篇 |
2002年 | 7篇 |
2001年 | 9篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1988年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1958年 | 1篇 |
1957年 | 2篇 |
1956年 | 1篇 |
1950年 | 1篇 |
1944年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有368条查询结果,搜索用时 9 毫秒
41.
42.
43.
Steven L. Blader Batia M. Wiesenfeld Marion Fortin Sara L. Wheeler-Smith 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2013
The present research explores third parties’ (e.g., jurors, ombudsmen, auditors, and employees observing others’ encounters) ability to objectively judge fairness. More specifically, the current research suggests that third parties’ justice judgments and reactions are biased by their attitudes toward the decision recipient and, in particular, the affective aspect of those attitudes as characterized by their felt social emotions. We explore how the congruence of a social emotion (i.e., the extent to which the emotion reflects feeling a subjective sense of alignment with the target of the emotion) can influence their evaluations of recipients’ decision outcomes. The five studies presented show that congruence can lead third parties to react positively to objectively unfair decision outcomes and, importantly, that the influence of social emotions on subjective justice judgments drive third party reactions to decisions, decision makers, and even national policies. 相似文献
44.
Marion Vorms 《Synthese》2013,190(2):293-319
Linkage (or genetic) maps are graphs, which are intended to represent the linear ordering of genes on the chromosomes. They are constructed on the basis of statistical data concerning the transmission of genes. The invention of this technique in 1913 was driven by Morgan’s group’s adoption of a set of hypotheses concerning the physical mechanism of heredity. These hypotheses were themselves grounded in Morgan’s defense of the chromosome theory of heredity, according to which chromosomes are the physical basis of genes. In this paper, I analyze the 1919 debate between William Castle and Morgan’s group, about the construction of genetic maps. The official issue of the debate concerns the arrangement of genes on chromosomes. However, the disputants tend to carry out the discussions about how one should model the data in order to draw predictions concerning the transmission of genes; the debate does not bear on the data themselves, nor does it focus on the hypotheses explaining these data. The main criteria that are appealed to by the protagonists are simplicity and predictive efficacy. However, I show that both parties’ assessments of the simplicity and predictive efficacy of different ways of modeling the data themselves depend on background theoretical positions. I aim at clarifying how preference for a given model and theoretical commitments articulate. 相似文献
45.
46.
Joseph Brozek Marion White McPherson 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》1973,8(4):236-244
This paper represents one step in the effort to locate, examine, and make generally available archival materials related to the life and work of I. P. Pavlov and held outside the Soviet Union, in particular in the United States. The Archives of the History of American Psychology contain fairly extensive correspondence among American psychologists, with informative references to Pavlov, including letters written by K. S. Lashley, R. M. Yerkes, and J. B. Watson. References to Pavlov are also located in a variety of other sources, including reminiscenses of psychologists and R. M. Yerkes’“Obituary” of Pavlov written in 1916. Pictures of Pavlov and his close associates are reproduced from still photographs and motion picture films. 相似文献
48.
Lt. Col. Marion W. Richardson 《Psychometrika》1944,9(4):245-248
The predictive efficiency of a test used to select personnel is defined in terms of total effectiveness of the group thus selected, as compared with chance selection. The formula developed requires the use of an estimate of the ratio of average effectiveness of men selected to the average effectiveness of men not selected by the test. The predictive efficiency of the test varies directly with the magnitude of this ratio and also directly with the percentage rejected. 相似文献
49.
Although creativity has drawn the attention of researchers during the past century, collaborative processes have barely been investigated. In this article, the collective dimension of a creative process is investigated, based on a dynamic and ecological approach that includes an affective component. Dynamic means that the creative process is no longer considered as sequential, and ecological, that the individuals were observed in their normal place of creation. This study was conducted with science students that were required to carry on a research project during 15 weeks in small groups. The results tend to confirm the hypothesis of a nonlinear process, with important variations of the affects depending on the stage. Moreover, each group creative process seems to be specific. The relation between the social functioning of the group and the process is also discussed. Finally, because it is a part of any teamwork, verbalization is proposed as a new powerful tool to investigate creative process. 相似文献
50.
Marion Müller 《Human Studies》2016,39(3):405-431
The segregation of the sexes in sport still seems to be regarded as a matter of course. In contrast to other performance classes, e.g., age and weight, which are constructed on the grounds of directly relevant performance features, in the case of gender it is dealt with the merely statistical factor that women on average perform less well than men. And yet unlike weight or age classes, which can be interchanged if the required performances are provided, the segregation between the sexes in many types of sport seems to be quite impermeable. Contrary to popular beliefs women were not excluded from the beginning of modern sport. Hence, the meaning of gender differences varies in the course of time and in types of sport. Thus, this paper firstly gives a short summary of the different relevance of gender differences in high performance competitions of various types of sport. Then the historical construction of gender as central dimension in modern sport will be illustrated by using the example of soccer. Here in this paper, the elaborate processes of reproducing this incommensurability between women and men in sport is the main focus: At first big sport organisations tried to exclude women whose performances were well above the expectations by establishing so called “Gender verifications” which suspected them of fraud. Although it became certain that there is no clear differentiation between the sexes, gender segregation was not abolished. Instead new regulations have been invented to treat and “normalise” people who are difficult to categorise, e.g., transsexuals and women with Hyperandrogenism, in order to subsume them into the existent gender categories. So finally, gender segregation in sport seems to be just another example for Goffman’s institutional reflexivity: While the separation of the competitions are said to be a natural consequence of the differences between men and women, it actually is just a tool to create those differences. 相似文献