首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   178690篇
  免费   7972篇
  国内免费   156篇
  2020年   2783篇
  2019年   3427篇
  2018年   3494篇
  2017年   3950篇
  2016年   4592篇
  2015年   3910篇
  2014年   4790篇
  2013年   23434篇
  2012年   4619篇
  2011年   3672篇
  2010年   3859篇
  2009年   4750篇
  2008年   3899篇
  2007年   3429篇
  2006年   4042篇
  2005年   4041篇
  2004年   3511篇
  2003年   3153篇
  2002年   2953篇
  2001年   3538篇
  2000年   3396篇
  1999年   3301篇
  1998年   2838篇
  1997年   2673篇
  1996年   2601篇
  1995年   2427篇
  1994年   2393篇
  1993年   2357篇
  1992年   2808篇
  1991年   2646篇
  1990年   2520篇
  1989年   2385篇
  1988年   2349篇
  1987年   2381篇
  1986年   2370篇
  1985年   2631篇
  1984年   2640篇
  1983年   2416篇
  1982年   2407篇
  1981年   2383篇
  1980年   2197篇
  1979年   2366篇
  1978年   2237篇
  1977年   2196篇
  1976年   2016篇
  1975年   2164篇
  1974年   2251篇
  1973年   2154篇
  1972年   1734篇
  1971年   1658篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
201.
202.
203.
204.
205.
Several studies have illuminated how processing manual action verbs (MaVs) affects the programming or execution of concurrent hand movements. Here, to circumvent key confounds in extant designs, we conducted the first assessment of motor–language integration during handwriting—a task in which linguistic and motoric processes are co‐substantiated. Participants copied MaVs, non‐manual action verbs, and non‐action verbs as we collected measures of motor programming and motor execution. Programming latencies were similar across conditions, but execution was faster for MaVs than for the other categories, regardless of whether word meanings were accessed implicitly or explicitly. In line with the Hand‐Action‐Network Dynamic Language Embodiment (HANDLE) model, such findings suggest that effector‐congruent verbs can prime manual movements even during highly automatized tasks in which motoric and verbal processes are naturally intertwined. Our paradigm opens new avenues for fine‐grained explorations of embodied language processes.  相似文献   
206.
207.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
208.
An electronic apparatus using infrared beams for monitoring the movements of individual bees under dark conditions is described. The searching behavior of workers in an arena was monitored over 2-h periods. Mean ambulatory velocity for one bee over a distance of 100 mm was 45.6±1.51 mm/sec (n=45). Thigmokinesis and temporal activity patterns are illustrated.  相似文献   
209.
210.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号