全文获取类型
收费全文 | 38832篇 |
免费 | 610篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
39444篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 441篇 |
2019年 | 553篇 |
2018年 | 725篇 |
2017年 | 795篇 |
2016年 | 816篇 |
2015年 | 565篇 |
2014年 | 692篇 |
2013年 | 2958篇 |
2012年 | 1257篇 |
2011年 | 1254篇 |
2010年 | 831篇 |
2009年 | 772篇 |
2008年 | 1129篇 |
2007年 | 1139篇 |
2006年 | 1024篇 |
2005年 | 909篇 |
2004年 | 840篇 |
2003年 | 806篇 |
2002年 | 824篇 |
2001年 | 1163篇 |
2000年 | 1121篇 |
1999年 | 869篇 |
1998年 | 386篇 |
1997年 | 380篇 |
1996年 | 353篇 |
1995年 | 363篇 |
1993年 | 358篇 |
1992年 | 736篇 |
1991年 | 652篇 |
1990年 | 704篇 |
1989年 | 612篇 |
1988年 | 641篇 |
1987年 | 599篇 |
1986年 | 608篇 |
1985年 | 551篇 |
1984年 | 514篇 |
1983年 | 490篇 |
1982年 | 359篇 |
1979年 | 573篇 |
1978年 | 410篇 |
1975年 | 454篇 |
1974年 | 501篇 |
1973年 | 532篇 |
1972年 | 399篇 |
1971年 | 389篇 |
1970年 | 352篇 |
1969年 | 401篇 |
1968年 | 474篇 |
1967年 | 432篇 |
1966年 | 356篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
751.
Analyses of interpersonal behavior within a borderline population usually focus on object relations variables. In contrast, this preliminary research focuses on the nature of attachment styles and its relation to behavioral pathology. 24 hospitalized borderline patients were administered an Attachment Style Inventory and Millon Clinical Multiaxial Inventory. Analysis confirms predicted differences in attachment style between the borderline and normal control samples as well as indicates that a dependent style of attachment within this borderline sample is associated with less pathology than an avoidant, hostile, or resistant/ambivalent style. The implications of this preliminary work for full-scale research are addressed. 相似文献
752.
Carroll and Chang have claimed that CANDECOMP applied to symmetric matrices yields equivalent coordinate matrices, as needed for INDSCAL. Although this claim has appeared to be valid for all practical purposes, it has gone without a rigorous mathematical footing. The purpose of the present paper is to clarify CANDECOMP in this respect. It is shown that equivalent coordinate matrices are not granted at global minima when the symmetric matrices are not Gramian, or when these matrices are Gramian but the solution not globally optimal.Part of this research has been supported by The Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO), PSYCHON-grant (560-267-011). 相似文献
753.
J. L. A. Garcia 《Synthese》1991,86(3):349-360
This paper criticizes the thesis that intending to do something is reducible to some combination of beliefs and desires. Against Audi's recent formulation of such a view I offer as counterexample a case wherein an agent who wants and expects to V has not yet decided whether to V and hence does not yet intend to. I try to show that whereas belief that one will V is not necessary for intending to V, as illustrated in cases of desperate attempts to V, one cannot intend to V without preferring to V (rather than not V) and thus one cannot intend to V without, in some sense, wanting to V (at least wanting it in preference to not V-ing). The connection of one's intentions with one's objectives, attempts, plans, and hopes is briefly treated, and some influential work by Davidson is criticized. 相似文献
754.
755.
H G Smid W Lamain M M Hogeboom G Mulder L J Mulder 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(3):696-714
This study was designed to test whether information transmission between the perceptual and motor levels occurs continuously or in discrete steps. Ss performed visual search across nontargets that shared visual features with one of two possible targets, each assigned to a different response. In addition to reaction time, psychophysiological measures were used to assess the duration of target search and the onset of central and peripheral motor activity. Nontargets sharing features with a target selectively activated the response associated with that target, even when it was not present in the display. This suggests that information transmission to the motor level can consist of fine-grained visual information and that visual search and response selection occur in parallel. 相似文献
756.
Task dynamics and resource dynamics in the assembly of a coordinated rhythmic activity. 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
G P Bingham R C Schmidt M T Turvey L D Rosenblum 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(2):359-381
Task dynamics corresponding to rhythmic movements emerge from interactions among dynamical resources composed of the musculature, the link segments, and the nervous and circulatory systems. This article investigated whether perturbations of interlimb coordination might be effect over circulatory and nervous elements. Stiffness of wrist-pendulums oscillated at a common tempo and at 180 degrees relative phase was perturbed through the use of tonic activity about an ankle. Left and right stiffnesses, the common period, and the phase relation all changed. Stiffnesses increased with ankle torque in proportion to the wrist's inertial load. Despite different changes in stiffness at the two wrists, isochrony was preserved. The stability was shown to be consistent with the proportionality of changes in stiffness to the inertial loads. The phase departed from antiphase in proportion to the asymmetry of inertial loads. The size of departures decreased with increasing ankle torque. An account was developed in terms of muscular, circulatory, and nervous functions. 相似文献
757.
758.
Twenty reading comprehension-disabled (CD) and 20 reading comprehension and word recognition-disabled (CWRD), right-handed male children were matched with 20 normal-achieving age-matched controls and 20 normal-achieving reading level-matched controls and tested for left ear report on dichotic listening tasks using digits and consonant-vowel combinations (CVs). Left ear report for CVs and digits did not correlate for any of the groups. Both reading-disabled groups showed lower left ear report on digits. On CVs the CD group showed a high left ear report but only when there were no priming precursors, such as directions to attend right first and to process digits first. Priming effects interfered with the processing of both digits and CVs. Theoretically, the CWRD group seems to be characterized by a depressed right hemisphere, whereas the CD group may have a more labile right hemisphere, perhaps tending to overengagement for CV tasks but vulnerable to situational precursors in the form of priming effects. Implications extend to (1) subtyping practices in research with the learning-disabled, (2) inferences drawn from studies using different dichotic stimuli, and (3) the neuropsychology of reading disorders. 相似文献
759.
L Hochhaus K M Marohn 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1991,17(2):422-432
A visual prime succeeded by a brief target produces a paradox. Namely, target repetition yields poorer identification accuracy and shorter duration judgments than unrelated prime-target pairs. Experiment 1 manipulated stimulus onset asynchrony to learn when repetition blindness is maximized. Experiments 2 and 3 manipulated expectancy of repetitions through changes in the proportion of repeated trials and instructions, respectively. Results indicate that repetition blindness is influenced by subject strategies and that the change is not mediated by response bias. Experiment 4 showed that increasing the spatial distance between prime and target reduced but did not eliminate repetition blindness. The current data support joint explanation of repetition blindness in terms of perceptual capture (prime-target fusion) and token individuation failure (problems in encoding episodic reoccurrences of an event). 相似文献
760.
Albino Sprague-Dawley rats with complete septal lesions and rats with control operations were studied under fixed-ratio (FR) schedules of reinforcement. Both groups were trained for 10 sessions each under FR 10, 20, 40, 60, 80, and 100. In contrast to findings from progressive FR studies and some simple FR studies, septal lesions resulted in lower overall and local response rates along with longer postreinforcement pauses. These effects were especially evident during the FR 100 schedule of reinforcement. A comparison of reinforcement rate as a function of FR size within the context of behavioral economics (i.e., a demand function) indicated that septal lesions did not alter the reward value of food. These findings suggest that responding on FR schedules of reinforcement can be altered by the various procedures used to train rats to reach the terminal value of a reinforcement schedule. 相似文献