全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1065篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
1075篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 10篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 16篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 22篇 |
2016年 | 39篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 30篇 |
2013年 | 126篇 |
2012年 | 47篇 |
2011年 | 57篇 |
2010年 | 27篇 |
2009年 | 23篇 |
2008年 | 38篇 |
2007年 | 29篇 |
2006年 | 45篇 |
2005年 | 27篇 |
2004年 | 19篇 |
2003年 | 30篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 15篇 |
2000年 | 20篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 19篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 17篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 16篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 16篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 16篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1973年 | 10篇 |
1968年 | 7篇 |
1967年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有1075条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
151.
Peter Fischer Andreas Kastenmüller Dieter Frey Claudia Peus 《Journal of applied social psychology》2009,39(1):42-61
In the workplace, people can experience various types of failure and frustrations resulting in spontaneous social comparisons with other colleagues who are more (upward comparison) or less (downward comparison) successful. Upward comparisons especially have been shown to increase envy toward the outperforming benchmark colleague, which could negatively affect social interaction with this colleague. In line with this, our results consistently show that upward comparisons are associated with higher levels of experienced envy, as well as with a lower transmission rate of high-quality information. By contrast, type of social comparison had no impact on the amount of transmitted low-quality, work-relevant information. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed. 相似文献
152.
Peter Fischer Eva Jonas Dieter Frey Stefan Schulz‐Hardt 《European journal of social psychology》2005,35(4):469-492
In research on selective exposure to information, people have been found to predominantly seek information supporting rather than conflicting with their opinion. In most of these studies, participants were allowed to search for as many pieces of information as they liked. However, in many situations, the amount of information that people can search for is restricted. We report four experiments addressing this issue. Experiment 1 suggests that objective limits regarding the maximum number of pieces of information the participants could search for increases the preference for selecting supporting over conflicting information. In Experiment 2, just giving participants a cue about information scarcity induces the same effect, even in the absence of any objective restrictions. Finally, Experiment 3 and 4 clarify the underlying psychological process by showing that information limits increase selective exposure to information because information search is guided by the expected information quality, which is basically biased towards supporting information, and information limits act to reinforce this tendency. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
153.
Marilyn J. Mason PhD 《Contemporary Family Therapy》1987,9(1-2):90-105
This paper describes the process in which families participate in a wilderness experience. Metaphors such as climbing or paddling a canoe are translated into daily living patterns to deepen individual and family self-knowledge, self-esteem, and intimacy. 相似文献
154.
155.
Marilyn H. Grantham Delwyn A. Dyer 《New Directions for Adult & Continuing Education》1981,1981(11):13-20
Community development as an educational process helps citizens understand the many facets of their community and learn how to obtain support to maintain or improve it. 相似文献
156.
157.
Peter Fischer Joachim I. Krueger Tobias Greitemeyer Kathrin Asal Nilüfer Aydin Evelyn Vingilis 《European Review of Social Psychology》2013,24(1):224-257
Media contents that glorify risk-taking (e.g., video games that simulate illegal street racing, TV depictions of dangerous extreme sports or stunts) are becoming increasingly popular. Theory and research suggest that risk glorification is associated with risk-taking among the audience. Until recently the evidence was limited in three ways: First, most studies, being correlational, did not permit clear causal inferences. Second, we did not know the strength of the effect of different types of media and different types of risk-taking outcomes. Third, early research did little to isolate the underlying psychological processes that mediate the pernicious effects of risk glorifying media formats. In order to address these open questions we review the results of an experimental programme of research and the findings of a comprehensive meta-analysis. The main findings are that (a) the risky media contents do indeed have causal force; (b) according to our meta-analysis active participation in risk glorifying media interfaces has a larger effect than passive consumption; (c) psychological processes include the priming of risk-related constructs, effects of risk-positive situational heuristic cues, perceived social norms, personal risk habituation, and changes in the recipient's self-concept. To integrate these findings we propose a theoretical framework derived from broad-range socio-cognitive models. We conclude with a discussion of possible directions for future research. 相似文献
158.
Andreas Kastenmüller Eva Jonas Peter Fischer Dieter Frey Julia Fischer 《Journal of applied social psychology》2013,43(1):138-146
Audience confirmation bias (ACB) refers to the extent to which people prefer information supporting (vs. conflicting with) their audience's views. In two studies, we showed that advisors shifted their ACB toward the needs of their advisees (i.e., audience): When advisors were led to believe that their advisees wanted to defend their views, the ACB was higher compared with when advisees were open minded for critique. Study 2 indicated that this pattern occurred because advisors wanted to have a pleasant interaction with their advisees (impression motivation): Whereas impression‐motivated advisors exhibited a stronger ACB when they were asked to give advice to a defensive (vs. open‐minded) advisee, accuracy‐motivated advisors showed a balanced ACB, regardless of their advisee's needs. 相似文献
159.
Terror management theory suggests that people cope with awareness of death by investing in some kind of literal or symbolic immortality. Given the centrality of death transcendence beliefs in most religions, the authors hypothesized that religious beliefs play a protective role in managing terror of death. The authors report three studies suggesting that affirming intrinsic religiousness reduces both death-thought accessibility following mortality salience and the use of terror management defenses with regard to a secular belief system. Study 1 showed that after a naturally occurring reminder of mortality, people who scored high on intrinsic religiousness did not react with worldview defense, whereas people low on intrinsic religiousness did. Study 2 specified that intrinsic religious belief mitigated worldview defense only if participants had the opportunity to affirm their religious beliefs. Study 3 illustrated that affirmation of religious belief decreased death-thought accessibility following mortality salience only for those participants who scored high on the intrinsic religiousness scale. Taken as a whole, these results suggest that only those people who are intrinsically vested in their religion derive terror management benefits from religious beliefs. 相似文献
160.
This study estimated the relationship between followers' locus of control and organizational citizenship behaviors for a sample of 375 government employees, whose M age was 46 yr. 53% were women; 42% earned bachelor's degrees. Analyses showed a significant positive correlation of .35 between followers' locus of control and their organizational behaviors. More complex models and research designs are necessary to generalize. 相似文献