全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4140篇 |
免费 | 67篇 |
专业分类
4207篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 41篇 |
2023年 | 51篇 |
2022年 | 71篇 |
2021年 | 100篇 |
2020年 | 138篇 |
2019年 | 135篇 |
2018年 | 200篇 |
2017年 | 216篇 |
2016年 | 185篇 |
2015年 | 119篇 |
2014年 | 140篇 |
2013年 | 539篇 |
2012年 | 272篇 |
2011年 | 271篇 |
2010年 | 185篇 |
2009年 | 107篇 |
2008年 | 229篇 |
2007年 | 171篇 |
2006年 | 152篇 |
2005年 | 100篇 |
2004年 | 87篇 |
2003年 | 100篇 |
2002年 | 67篇 |
2001年 | 35篇 |
2000年 | 48篇 |
1999年 | 42篇 |
1998年 | 36篇 |
1997年 | 30篇 |
1996年 | 23篇 |
1995年 | 14篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 20篇 |
1992年 | 17篇 |
1991年 | 19篇 |
1990年 | 11篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 14篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 20篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 10篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 9篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 7篇 |
1965年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有4207条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
261.
Although homework completion is said to be rather important to achievement, nowadays there is a growing concern of educators about the increasing number of students who do not engage properly on doing the homework tasks and the subsequent impact on school failure rates. Focusing on English as a Foreign Language (EFL) and using a sample of 591 Portuguese fifth and sixth graders, the present study analyses the role played by a number of homework variables on students' achievement (proximal and distal), and their mediating role on the use of self-regulated learning strategies and perceived self-efficacy in the domain. Data confirm the indirect effect of homework on school achievement, by means of the referred cognitive and motivational variables (use of self-regulated learning strategies and self-efficacy). These findings are further discussed in order to highlight the significant role homework completion can play on fighting school failure. 相似文献
262.
This paper presents two experiments on nutrient-based flavor conditioning with rats as subjects and sucrose as the unconditioned stimulus (US). Experiment 1 was aimed at establishing an optimal control for conditioning, comparing simultaneous and serial presentations of a flavor and the US. The results showed that simultaneous, but not serial training, produced conditioning. Experiment 2 was designed to obtain evidence of summation as an index of both conditioned inhibition and predictive learning. Group Simultaneous received Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training during which flavor A was simultaneously paired with sucrose on excitatory trials (A+), and forming an unreinforced compound with flavor B on inhibitory trials (AB-). An independent excitor for the summation test was also trained by simultaneous pairings with sucrose (C+). In the control group (Blocked), the AB- trials were presented forming a block at the beginning of training to avoid a negative contingency-relationship with sucrose, and flavor A received serial rather than simultaneous pairing with sucrose (A --> +). On the summation test, only in group Simultaneous was consumption of the CB compound lower than that of flavor C alone, suggesting that, during training, flavor A activated an expectancy of the US occurrence. 相似文献
263.
Fernández J Quiroga MA del Olmo I Aróztegui J Martín A 《The Spanish journal of psychology》2011,14(2):899-911
This study was designed to develop a computerized test to assess gender roles. This test is presented as a decision-making task to mask its purpose. Each item displays a picture representing an activity and a brief sentence that describes it. Participants have to choose the most suitable sex to perform each activity: man or woman. The test (Gender Roles Test, GRT-36) consists of 36 items/activities. The program registers both the choices made and their response times (RTs). Responses are considered as stereotyped when the chosen sex fits stereotyped roles and non-stereotyped when the chosen sex does not fit stereotyped roles. Individual means (RTs) were computed for stereotyped and non-stereotyped responses, differentiating between domestic and work spheres. A "D" score, reflecting the strength of association between activities and sex, was calculated for each sphere and sex. The study incorporated 78 participants (69% women and 31% men) ranging from 19 to 59 years old. The results show that: (a) reading speed does not explain the variability in the RTs; (b) RTs show good internal consistency; (c) RTs are shorter for stereotyped than for neutral stimuli; (d) RTs are shorter for stereotyped than for non-stereotyped responses. Intended goals are supported by obtained results. Scores provided by the task facilitate both group and individual detailed analysis of gender role, differentiating the gender role assigned to men from that assigned to women, at the domestic and work spheres. Obtained data fall within the scope of the genderology and their implications are discussed. 相似文献
264.
García García J Ortega Campos E De la Fuente Sánchez L 《The Spanish journal of psychology》2011,14(2):1050-1055
In 1999, Wilkinson and the Task Force on Statistical Inference published "Statistical Methods and Psychology: Guidelines and Explanation." The authors made several recommendations about how to improve the quality of Psychology research papers. One of these was to report some effect-size index in the results of the research. In 2001, the fifth edition of the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association included this recommendation. In Spain, in 2003, scientific journals like Psicothema or the International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology (IJCHP) published editorials and papers expressing the need to calculate the effect size in the research papers. The aim of this study is to determine whether the papers published from 2003 to 2008 in the four Spanish journals indexed in the Journal Citation Reports have reported some effect-size index of their results. The findings indicate that, in general, the followup of the norm has been scanty, though the evolution over the analyzed period is different depending on the journal. 相似文献
265.
Brascamp JW Blake R Kristjánsson Á 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2011,37(6):1700-1707
With attention and eye-movements humans orient to targets of interest. This orienting occurs faster when the same target repeats: priming of pop-out (PoP). While reaction times (RTs) can be important, PoP's real function could be to steer where to orient, a possibility underexposed in many current paradigms, as these predesignate a target to which to orient. In a novel procedure we intermixed pop-out trials (one oddball target, two identical distractors) with choice trials (one item of each kind) where observers freely chose an item to attend to. Pop-out trials strongly drove subsequent choice: observers typically chose the preceding target. Conversely, choice trials affected subsequent pop-out RTs. Conventional PoP measures correlated positively with our choice measures among observers, suggesting common mechanisms. Our results support PoP accounts centered on altered target priority, and underscore PoP's importance for visual exploration. 相似文献
266.
Castel AD Humphreys KL Lee SS Galván A Balota DA McCabe DP 《Developmental psychology》2011,47(6):1553-1564
Although attentional control and memory change considerably across the life span, no research has examined how the ability to strategically remember important information (i.e., value-directed remembering) changes from childhood to old age. The present study examined this in different age groups across the life span (N = 320, 5-96 years old). A selectivity task was used in which participants were asked to study and recall items worth different point values in order to maximize their point score. This procedure allowed for measures of memory quantity/capacity (number of words recalled) and memory efficiency/selectivity (the recall of high-value items relative to low-value items). Age-related differences were found for memory capacity, as young adults recalled more words than the other groups. However, in terms of selectivity, younger and older adults were more selective than adolescents and children. The dissociation between these measures across the life span illustrates important age-related differences in terms of memory capacity and the ability to selectively remember high-value information. 相似文献
267.
This two-wave longitudinal study examines the ability of life satisfaction and adjustment strategies to predict anxious and
depressive symptoms in coronary heart disease male patients. Studies have shown that most heart attack survivors report these
symptoms, which may worsen the prognosis of the disease. At Time 1, immediately after the first cardiac episode, eighty-eight
men reported their life satisfaction levels, adjustment strategies used, and anxious and depressive symptoms experienced.
At Time 2, six months later, sixty-three of those patients reported only their anxious and depressive symptoms again. The
results showed that, after controlling for demographic variables, anxious and depressive symptoms at Time 1 were predicted
by positive adjustment and life satisfaction. At Time 2, after controlling for both demographic variables and Time 1-emotional
symptoms, none of the psychological variables predicted anxious symptoms, while depressive symptoms were only predicted by
life satisfaction. It is concluded that an adequate level of life satisfaction may help to decrease emotional distress, both
short and long term, while the use of positive adjustment strategies is especially important immediately after diagnosis. 相似文献
268.
Different approaches to expertise and argumentation are discussed. After introducing the problem of expertise and its present
day significance in a historical context, various connections with the study of arguments are highlighted. The need for and
potential of argumentation analysis to contribute to existing research in social epistemology, science studies, and cognitive
science, is discussed, touching on the problems of reasoning and argumentation, embodiment, tacit knowledge, expert context
versus public context, expert disagreement, persuasion versus justification, and argument analysis as meta-expertise. As the
arguments used by experts constitute a boundary object, we presume that a dialogue format is suitable to address central problems
of the special issue “Rethinking Arguments from Experts”. 相似文献
269.
This study presents a database of 500 words from five semantic categories: animals, body parts, furniture, clothing, and intelligence.
Each category contains 100 words, and data on lexical availability, age of acquisition, imageability, typicality, concept
familiarity, written word frequency, and word length in number of syllables are provided with each word. The full set of norms
may be downloaded from www.psychonomic.org/archive. 相似文献
270.
Canals J Hernández-Martínez C Fernández-Ballart JD 《Infant behavior & development》2011,34(1):152-160
The aim of this study was to analyze the extent to which the relations and the stability of the neonatal behaviour and the infant behaviour at both 4 and 12 months could be used to predict the infant temperament at 6 years. Seventy-two full-term, normal-weight, healthy children were followed from 3 days postpartum to 6 years. Neonatal behaviour at 3 and 30 days was assessed using the Neonatal Behaviour Assessment Scale (NBAS), behaviour at 4 and 12 months was assessed using the Infant Behaviour Record (IBR) from the Bayley Scales for Infant Development (BSID) and temperament at 6 years was assessed using the Dimensions of Temperament Survey-Revised (DOTS-R). Negative and positive affectivity and attention were the factors extracted from the behaviours of the IBR. Our results showed a low/moderate correlation between neonatal behaviour on the one hand and behaviour at 4 and 12 months and temperament at 6 years on the other hand. The motor performance, orientation and ANS stability of the neonate at 3 and 30 days were predictors of positive and negative affectivity at 4 months. State regulation and ANS stability at 3 days were predictors of negative affectivity and attention respectively at 12 months. Negative affectivity at 12 months and endurance at 30 days were predictors of general activity and persistence/attention at 6 years. Also, an inverse relation was found between state regulation at 30 days and general activity at 6 years, and a positive relation was found between attention at 4 months and persistence/attention at 6 years. We conclude that neonatal behaviour and behaviour in the first year of life are good predictors of temperament at 6 years and that early negative affectivity has an important role on infant temperament development. 相似文献