全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8799篇 |
免费 | 439篇 |
国内免费 | 4篇 |
专业分类
9242篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 64篇 |
2022年 | 65篇 |
2021年 | 100篇 |
2020年 | 158篇 |
2019年 | 193篇 |
2018年 | 238篇 |
2017年 | 268篇 |
2016年 | 273篇 |
2015年 | 200篇 |
2014年 | 214篇 |
2013年 | 870篇 |
2012年 | 350篇 |
2011年 | 364篇 |
2010年 | 230篇 |
2009年 | 202篇 |
2008年 | 269篇 |
2007年 | 263篇 |
2006年 | 269篇 |
2005年 | 201篇 |
2004年 | 200篇 |
2003年 | 191篇 |
2002年 | 171篇 |
2001年 | 250篇 |
2000年 | 223篇 |
1999年 | 185篇 |
1998年 | 65篇 |
1993年 | 70篇 |
1992年 | 128篇 |
1991年 | 124篇 |
1990年 | 122篇 |
1989年 | 108篇 |
1988年 | 101篇 |
1987年 | 105篇 |
1986年 | 113篇 |
1985年 | 114篇 |
1984年 | 100篇 |
1983年 | 81篇 |
1981年 | 70篇 |
1979年 | 117篇 |
1978年 | 64篇 |
1976年 | 67篇 |
1975年 | 86篇 |
1974年 | 109篇 |
1973年 | 96篇 |
1972年 | 80篇 |
1971年 | 88篇 |
1970年 | 84篇 |
1969年 | 86篇 |
1968年 | 82篇 |
1967年 | 94篇 |
排序方式: 共有9242条查询结果,搜索用时 93 毫秒
11.
12.
A comparison of two measures of egocentrism 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
L D Cohn S G Millstein C E Irwin N E Adler S M Kegeles P Dolcini G Stone 《Journal of personality assessment》1988,52(2):212-222
This study examined the relationship between two prominent measures of egocentrism, Enright's Adolescent Egocentrism Scale (AES) and Elkind's Imaginary Audience Scale (IAS), in a sample of 458 adolescents, between 10 and 15 years old. Both correlational and factor analyses indicated that the two measures assess distinctly different phenomena. As predicted, IAS scores were correlated with self-reported levels of shyness, nervousness, and social skills. These results provide partial evidence for the validity of the IAS as a measure of self-consciousness, though not necessarily egocentrism per se. The validity of the AES was not examined. 相似文献
13.
Effects of Social Demand on Reports of Self-Monitored Blood Glucose in Adolescents with Type 1 Diabetes Mellitus 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Alan M. Delamater Steven M. Kurtz Neil H. White Julio V. Santiago 《Journal of applied social psychology》1988,18(6):491-502
The purpose of this study was to test the hypothesis that social demand could substantially affect reports of self-monitored blood glucose (BG) in adolescents with insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. Of 34 patients initially enrolled in the study, 10 were excluded because they did not bring any BG records with them to an outpatient clinic appointment. The remaining 24 patients were randomly assigned to either a low or high social demand condition that provided instructions for monitoring of BG for the week following the appointment. The subjects' BG records were quantified to provide frequency of measurement and mean reported BG for the week prior to and after the clinic visit. Five subjects did not return their BG records for the week following the intervention. The analyses were therefore based on the 19 subjects from whom complete records were obtained. The 12 subjects in the low social demand group and 7 subjects in the high social demand group were equivalent with regard to age, duration of diabetes, socioeconomic status, and glycosylated hemoglobin. Frequency of BG measurement was similar in both groups during both weeks. The mean BG value reported in the week prior to intervention was similar for the groups. However, analyses of the post-intervention BGs revealed that subjects in the low-demand group reported significantly higher BGs compared to pre-intervention and to subjects in the high-demand group. These findings suggest that self-monitoring and reporting of BG is a social behavior that is affected by the demand characteristics of the interpersonal patient-health provider relationship. Because optimal treatment planning for individuals with diabetes requires accurate BG records, care must be taken to interpret them in light of the social demand characteristics associated with clinical assessment. 相似文献
14.
15.
16.
V E Whiffen 《Journal of abnormal psychology》1988,97(4):467-474
17.
Wolfgang Hell Gerd Gigerenzer Siegfried Gauggel Maria Mall Michael Müller 《Memory & cognition》1988,16(6):533-538
If subjects are asked to recollect a former response after having been informed about the correct response, their recollection tends to approach the correct response. This effect has been termedhindsight bias. We studied hindsight bias in an experiment requiring numerical responses to almanac-type questions for physical quantities. We varied (1) the time at which the correct information was provided, (2) the encoding of the original responses by asking/not asking subjects to give a reason for the respective response, and (3) the motivation to recall correctly. We found that hindsight is less biased if reasons are given and if the correct information is provided at an earlier time. Motivation had only interactive effects: (1) With high motivation to recall correctly, the time the correct information was provided had no influence. (2) With reasons given, the variation of motivation showed no effect. These results rule out purely motivational and purely automatic explanations. 相似文献
18.
There is some controversy concerning whether or not the visual abilities of the newborn are mediated entirely through subcortical pathways or whether the visual cortex is functioning at birth. A critical test of cortical functioning is discrimination of orientation: orientation-selective neurons are found in the visual cortex but not in subcortical parts of the visual system. An experiment is described in which newborn infants were habituated to a square-wave grating oriented 45 degrees from vertical. After habituation, significant preferences for the novel, mirror-image, grating were found, a result which argues for some degree of visual cortical functioning at birth. 相似文献
19.
V Di Lollo P Dixon 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1988,14(4):671-681
Iconic memory, which was initially regarded as a unitary phenomenon, has since been subdivided into several components. In the present work we examined the joint effects of two such components (visible persistence and the visual analog representation) on performance in a partial report task. The display consisted of 15 alphabetic characters arranged around the perimeter of an imaginary circle on the face of an oscilloscope. The observer named the character singled out by a bar-probe. Two factors were varied: exposure duration of the array (10, 50, 100, 150, 200, 300, 400 or 500 ms) and duration of blank period (interstimulus interval, ISI) between the termination of the array and the onset of the probe (0, 50, 100, 150, or 200 ms). Performance was progressively impaired as both exposure duration and ISI were increased. The results were explained in terms of a probabilistic combinatorial model in which the timecourses of visible persistence and of the visual analog representation are regarded as time-locked to the onset and to the end of stimulation, respectively. The impairing effect of exposure duration was attributed to the relatively high spatial demands of the task that could be met optimally by information in visible persistence (which declines as a function of exposure duration), but less adequately by information in the visual analog representation. A second experiment, employing a task with lesser spatial demands, confirmed this interpretation. 相似文献
20.
In this pilot study, 20 middle-school-age children classified as emotionally handicapped were administered Forms L and M of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test--Revised in test-retest fashion. Pearson correlations for Form L were .90, for Form M, .69, and these dependent correlations were significantly different from each other. As triennial school psychological evaluations typically contain tests which have been administered previously, e.g., WISC-R, WRAT-R, we suggest that psychologists use caution when using Form M to test or retest the receptive vocabulary of emotionally handicapped or disturbed middle-school-age children. 相似文献