全文获取类型
收费全文 | 17613篇 |
免费 | 842篇 |
国内免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
18464篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 249篇 |
2019年 | 275篇 |
2018年 | 375篇 |
2017年 | 380篇 |
2016年 | 414篇 |
2015年 | 295篇 |
2014年 | 358篇 |
2013年 | 1362篇 |
2012年 | 603篇 |
2011年 | 555篇 |
2010年 | 385篇 |
2009年 | 340篇 |
2008年 | 507篇 |
2007年 | 525篇 |
2006年 | 468篇 |
2005年 | 431篇 |
2004年 | 376篇 |
2003年 | 328篇 |
2002年 | 339篇 |
2001年 | 702篇 |
2000年 | 683篇 |
1999年 | 475篇 |
1998年 | 176篇 |
1997年 | 173篇 |
1996年 | 176篇 |
1995年 | 151篇 |
1992年 | 374篇 |
1991年 | 346篇 |
1990年 | 365篇 |
1989年 | 307篇 |
1988年 | 292篇 |
1987年 | 287篇 |
1986年 | 267篇 |
1985年 | 263篇 |
1984年 | 242篇 |
1983年 | 208篇 |
1982年 | 158篇 |
1979年 | 224篇 |
1978年 | 174篇 |
1977年 | 152篇 |
1975年 | 193篇 |
1974年 | 231篇 |
1973年 | 229篇 |
1972年 | 184篇 |
1971年 | 181篇 |
1970年 | 151篇 |
1969年 | 176篇 |
1968年 | 211篇 |
1967年 | 192篇 |
1966年 | 156篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
31.
A "change-of-standard" perspective on the relations among context, judgment, and memory 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
We conducted three studies that tested a "change-of-standard" perspective on the relations among context, judgment, and recall. Each study consisted of two or three sessions held a few days apart. All subjects read about the sentencing decisions of one or two target trial judges and of six nontarget trial judges who consistently gave either higher or lower sentences than the target judge(s). Each study varied both the standard that was available when subjects initially judged the sentencing decisions of a target judge and the standard available when subjects subsequently recalled those decisions. To accomplish this, we varied the context of judgment, the timing of judgment, and the overall category norm for trial judges' sentencing decisions that was available at recall. We found that although subjects had been exposed to the same target information and had initially judged it in the same way, their recall of the information was different depending on whether and how a change-of-standard had occurred between judgment and recall. Unique predictions of the change-of-standard perspective were confirmed that could not be accounted for in terms of other types of context effects on judgment and memory. 相似文献
32.
J T Cacioppo J S Martzke R E Petty L G Tassinary 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,54(4):592-604
Previous research has demonstrated that mild negative emotional imagery and unpleasant sensory stimuli lead to greater electromyographic activity over the brow muscle region than mild positive imagery and stimuli, even in the absence of significant changes in visceral and general facial EMG activity. Previous research has not addressed whether electromyographic responses over the brow region are a sensitive and specific index of emotions, however, since a multiplicity of events lead to changes in brow activity. In this research, facial electromyographic and audiovisual recordings were obtained while individuals were interviewed about themselves. Afterwards, individuals were asked to describe what they had been thinking of during specific segments of the interview marked by distinctive electromyographic responses over the brow region in the context of ongoing but stable levels of activity elsewhere in the face. The results are interpreted in terms of a continuous flow hypothesis of affect-laden information processing. 相似文献
33.
Some observations on control by spoken words in children's conditional discrimination and matching by exclusion 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
This study examined matching-to-sample procedures that might result in the emergence of conditional behavior never explicitly taught. Subjects were preschool children. Two pictures were displayed as comparisons on every trial, and samples were spoken words. In baseline training preceding each of three experiments, children learned to select pictures of a dog, a table, and a banana in response to their spoken English names. Thereafter, probe trials displayed novel comparisons with baseline comparisons: one novel comparison was displayed with the dog and another with the table. The three experiments differed primarily in the nature of the samples presented on probe trials. In Experiment 1, probe samples were novel words, "JAIJAI" and "BREEL." On the probes, each of seven subjects reliably selected the novel comparisons, apparently "excluding" the familiar ones. In Experiment 2, probe samples were from the subjects' baseline. On one probe, for example, the sample was "TABLE," and the subject had to choose either the dog or the novel picture. Exclusion was logically possible because the dog had always before been incorrect in the presence of "TABLE." Under these conditions, however, only two of nine children excluded reliably. In Experiment 3, probe samples were words that had never been samples on any matching-to-sample trial, but that had controlled the children's behavior in other settings. On one probe, for example, the sample was "PENCIL," and the subject had to choose either the dog or a novel picture. Subjects virtually always excluded the former and selected the latter. Unreliable exclusion in Experiment 2, therefore, apparently resulted because the probe samples had previously served also as samples on baseline trials. Spontaneous verbalizations recorded during probing provided further data consistent with this interpretation. The study helps to define variables controlling exclusion performances by showing that such performances are more likely to occur if the sample has no prior experimental history. 相似文献
34.
35.
36.
Wolfgang Hell Gerd Gigerenzer Siegfried Gauggel Maria Mall Michael Müller 《Memory & cognition》1988,16(6):533-538
If subjects are asked to recollect a former response after having been informed about the correct response, their recollection tends to approach the correct response. This effect has been termedhindsight bias. We studied hindsight bias in an experiment requiring numerical responses to almanac-type questions for physical quantities. We varied (1) the time at which the correct information was provided, (2) the encoding of the original responses by asking/not asking subjects to give a reason for the respective response, and (3) the motivation to recall correctly. We found that hindsight is less biased if reasons are given and if the correct information is provided at an earlier time. Motivation had only interactive effects: (1) With high motivation to recall correctly, the time the correct information was provided had no influence. (2) With reasons given, the variation of motivation showed no effect. These results rule out purely motivational and purely automatic explanations. 相似文献
37.
Two experiments investigated the effects of successive reinforcement contexts on choice. In the first, concurrent variable-interval schedules of primary reinforcement operated during the initial links of concurrent chains. The rate of this reinforcement arranged by the concurrent schedules was decreased across conditions: When it was higher than the terminal-link rate, preference for the higher frequency initial-link schedule increased relative to baseline. (During baseline, a standard concurrent-schedule procedure was in effect). When the initial-link reinforcement rate was lower than the terminal-link rate, preference converged toward indifference. In the second experiment, a chain schedule was available on a third key while a concurrent schedule was in effect on the side keys. When the terminal link of the chain schedule was produced, the side keys became inoperative. Availability of the chain schedule did not affect choice between the concurrent schedules. These results show that only when successive reinforcement contexts are produced by choice responding do those successive contexts affect choice in concurrent schedules. 相似文献
38.
39.
E Schier T Yecunnoamlack T Tegegne 《Psychiatrie, Neurologie, und medizinische Psychologie》1988,40(8):490-499
Of 1240 first-time consultations in the neuropsychiatric clinic of the Gondar College of Medical Sciences in an eleven-month period during 1985 and 1986, 152 patients came seeking relief from seizures. This paper deals with the origins and background of diagnostic, differential diagnostic, and therapeutic problems, and with problems of social rehabilitation. While in such a tradition-bound society certain hypotheses continue to have an unfavorable effect, there are unmistakable signs of positive change against a background of rapid social change. Conclusions are drawn for the better management of epileptic patients in developing countries. 相似文献
40.