全文获取类型
收费全文 | 34230篇 |
免费 | 643篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
34879篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 425篇 |
2019年 | 534篇 |
2018年 | 701篇 |
2017年 | 705篇 |
2016年 | 788篇 |
2015年 | 517篇 |
2014年 | 627篇 |
2013年 | 2670篇 |
2012年 | 1114篇 |
2011年 | 1107篇 |
2010年 | 678篇 |
2009年 | 725篇 |
2008年 | 928篇 |
2007年 | 952篇 |
2006年 | 857篇 |
2005年 | 722篇 |
2004年 | 706篇 |
2003年 | 643篇 |
2002年 | 646篇 |
2001年 | 1075篇 |
2000年 | 1068篇 |
1999年 | 794篇 |
1998年 | 331篇 |
1997年 | 311篇 |
1995年 | 323篇 |
1992年 | 637篇 |
1991年 | 593篇 |
1990年 | 577篇 |
1989年 | 551篇 |
1988年 | 544篇 |
1987年 | 485篇 |
1986年 | 491篇 |
1985年 | 533篇 |
1984年 | 439篇 |
1983年 | 404篇 |
1982年 | 311篇 |
1979年 | 479篇 |
1978年 | 330篇 |
1977年 | 310篇 |
1976年 | 322篇 |
1975年 | 366篇 |
1974年 | 435篇 |
1973年 | 466篇 |
1972年 | 402篇 |
1971年 | 343篇 |
1970年 | 325篇 |
1969年 | 337篇 |
1968年 | 421篇 |
1967年 | 371篇 |
1966年 | 359篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
William C. Rein 《Behavioral sciences & the law》1984,2(2):179-193
In September 1981, a symposium of distinguished mental health practitioners from various clinical, administrative, research, and legal disciplines was conducted on the topic of “Behavioral Science and the Secret Service,” under the sponsorship of the National Academy of Sciences, Institute of Medicine. The article combines a study of the symposium's written conference summary, the author's experience as legal counsel to a state security hospital, and research into federal cases that have interpreted the so-called “Presidential Threat Statute” (18 USC 871) in order to discuss the role of the Secret Service in protecting elected federal officials from physical harm. The potential for increasing practical cooperation between the Service and state mental health systems is also discussed. 相似文献
242.
243.
244.
To evaluate a contingency interpretation of conditioned inhibition (CI), rats were given “explicity unpaired” training in which the locus and duration of a CS within the inter-US (shock) interval were systematically manipulated for different groups. Summation and retardation tests in Experiment 1 indicated that stronger CI resulted from both a backward and a trace CS than from a midlocus CS of equal or greater duration. Complementing these findings, the same tests in Experiment 2 showed that, by comparison with novel-stimulus controls, CI developed to a trace CS but not to a mid-locus CS, nor to a trace CS that was accompanied by an immediate signal for the US. These findings argue against a contingency interpretation of CI and favor a contiguity interpretation stressing the short-term rehearsal of stimulus events. Such rehearsal of the US allows a backward CS, but not a mid-locus CS with an extended US-CS interval, to be discriminated as a signal for nonreinforcement, and thus to develop as a conditioned inhibitor. Similarly, excitatory conditioning to the memory trace of a CS allows the nominal trace CS to develop as a signal for nonreinforcement, and thus as a conditioned inhibitor, but not when its memory trace is overshadowed by another CS that immediately precedes the US. In short, the development of CI is facilitated when excitation is mediated by the memorial processing of either the US or a discrete CS for the US rather than by contextual cues. 相似文献
245.
Four adult male rats were each placed for three hours daily into an apparatus that provided individual compartments for six separate location-defined responses. The available responses consisted of: (1) the opportunity to turn off room lighting, producing darkness; (2) the opportunity to view a female rat; (3) the opportunity to turn off white noise; (4) the opportunity to drink; (5) the opportunity to eat; and (6) "other," representing time in the hallway between compartments. Each subject underwent a series of conditions characterized as an A-B-A-C-A design. Manipulations consisted of the removal of a low-probability response (darkness) and of a high-probability response (escape from noise) in a counter-balanced manner across subjects. The dependent measure for all subjects was the percentage of total session time spent in each compartment. Four predictive rules concerning the redistribution of behavior after response restriction were tested, including the constant-ratio rule, equal time redistribution, the most probable alternative, and the sequential-dependency rule. The results indicate no support for any of the four predictive rules and suggest that empirical assessment of restriction effects is necessary in reinforcement studies involving temporally extended responses. 相似文献
246.
Unlike earlier tests of an oversimplified version of this model, the validity of W. Mobley's (1977, Journal of Applied Psychology, 62, 237-240) original turnover model was fully investigated. Constructs that were neglected in prior studies were assessed and previously examined constructs were operationalized with more reliable measures. Measures of all constructs in Mobley's model were obtained from a survey of 192 hospital employees. Turnover data were collected a year following survey administration. Following the theoretical causal ordering of Mobley's constructs, each construct was regressed on all causally prior constructs. In general, each construct was accurately predicted by the linear combination of predictors representing its causal determinants. In the majority of instances, the best predictor of a model construct was the construct's immediate causal antecedent. Further, an alternative model was evaluated and compared with Mobley's model using path analysis. 相似文献
247.
248.
249.
250.