全文获取类型
收费全文 | 43832篇 |
免费 | 458篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
44291篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 260篇 |
2020年 | 368篇 |
2019年 | 505篇 |
2018年 | 3903篇 |
2017年 | 3292篇 |
2016年 | 2748篇 |
2015年 | 621篇 |
2014年 | 611篇 |
2013年 | 2283篇 |
2012年 | 1450篇 |
2011年 | 3234篇 |
2010年 | 2986篇 |
2009年 | 1927篇 |
2008年 | 2432篇 |
2007年 | 2853篇 |
2006年 | 733篇 |
2005年 | 787篇 |
2004年 | 743篇 |
2003年 | 657篇 |
2002年 | 546篇 |
2001年 | 586篇 |
2000年 | 653篇 |
1999年 | 427篇 |
1998年 | 274篇 |
1997年 | 214篇 |
1996年 | 218篇 |
1995年 | 204篇 |
1994年 | 176篇 |
1993年 | 185篇 |
1992年 | 313篇 |
1991年 | 327篇 |
1990年 | 349篇 |
1989年 | 307篇 |
1988年 | 303篇 |
1987年 | 264篇 |
1986年 | 281篇 |
1985年 | 296篇 |
1984年 | 224篇 |
1983年 | 235篇 |
1979年 | 270篇 |
1978年 | 234篇 |
1975年 | 225篇 |
1974年 | 255篇 |
1973年 | 277篇 |
1972年 | 233篇 |
1971年 | 225篇 |
1969年 | 204篇 |
1968年 | 264篇 |
1967年 | 251篇 |
1966年 | 220篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
242.
243.
The present paper selectively reviews the literature which compares full-term and preterm infants' communication with their mothers in the first year of life. The review is focused not only on what differences are found between full-term and preterm infants, but also why such differences exist. Particular emphasis is placed on the importance of interpreting the findings in the light of the developmental phase of the infants, the heterogeneity of the preterm population and the situation in which communication is studied. Most studies were not designed to consider these issues in any detail. Differences with respect to these factors can be found between studies, however, which may have contributed to the often contradictory findings. Consequently, comparisons between these studies provide clues about the effects of these factors on communicative development. The ultimate goal of this review is to point to important directions for future research on early communication in preterm infants. 相似文献
244.
Marek Franěk Jiři Mates Tomáš Radil Karin Beck Ernst Pöppel 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1994,55(2):204-217
Musically trained and untrained subjects (N=30) were asked to synchronize their finger tapping with stimuli in auditory patterns. Each pattern comprised six successive tonal stimuli of the same duration, the first of which was accented by a different frequency. The duration of interstimulus onset intervals (ISIs) gradually increased or decreased in constant steps toward the end of the patterns. Four values of such steps were used in different trials: 20, 30, 45, and 60 msec. Various time-control mechanisms are hypothesized as being simultaneously responsible for subjects’ incorrect reproduction of the internal temporal ratios of the stimulus patterns. The mechanism of assimilation (of a central tendency) led subjects to enforce a regular (isochronous) structure on the patterns. The influence of other time-control mechanisms (distinction, subjective expression of an accent, sequential transfer) was expressed mainly in differences between intertap onset intervals (ITIs) and the corresponding ISIs at the beginning of the patterns. The duration of the first two ITIs was in the majority of the trials in an inverse ratio to the ratio of the respective ISIs. The distortions resulting from the timing mechanisms concerned were more pronounced in the performance of nonmusicians than in that of musicians. 相似文献
245.
Previous research has shown that a task-irrelevant sudden onset of an object will capture an observer’s visual attention or draw it to that object (e.g., Yantis & Jonides, 1984). However, further research has demonstrated the apparent inability of an object with a task-irrelevant but unique color or luminance to capture attention (Jonides & Yantis, 1988). In the experiments reported here, we reexplore the question of whether task-irrelevant properties other than sudden onset may capture attention. Our results suggest that uniquely colored or luminous objects, as well as salient though irrelevant boundaries, do not appear to capture attention. However, these irrelevant features do appear to serve as landmarks for a top-down search strategy which becomes increasingly likely with larger display set sizes. These findings are described in terms of stimulus-driven and goal-directed aspects of attentional control. 相似文献
246.
247.
248.
249.
Peter Suedfeld Brian de Vries Susan Bluck Alistair B. C. Wallbaum Paul W. Schmidt 《International journal of psychology》1996,31(5):177-190
Although there are measurable differences in integrative complexity among solutions that individuals generate in dealing with problems, it is uncertain to what extent people comprehend, recognize, and have preferences among different levels of complexity. Integrative complexity is a function of differentiation (the perception of several attributes within, or perspectives about, a topic) and integration (combining the differentiated characteristics in an interactive or synthesizing solution). The current paper reports two experiments dealing with how university students perceive, interpret, and choose among solutions differing in complexity. Experiment 1 showed that subjects accurately rated the complexity of described solutions differing along the continuum, but that their assessment of their own responses differed from the results of expert scoring. Their self-estimated complexity was highly correlated with their preferences, and preferred complexity was reliably higher than either expert- or self-assessed complexity of subject-generated solutions. Subjects were able to hypothesize quite accurately about environmental and endogenous factors likely to affect complexity. Experiment 2 found that in response to problem scenarios, solutions selected as being potentially most effective were consistently more complex than solutions that participants considered themselves most likely to use. The idea of complexity seems to be intuitively recognizable and understandable by untrained subjects: They can and do distinguish among problem solutions (self-generated or presented) that vary on that dimension, and are able to assess accurately the effects of relevant variables. Such subjects also share the bias shown by experts in favour of the superiority of more complex approaches. 相似文献
250.
Abstract: Do memories change as we acquire new information? Recent research on memory distortion using implicit tests along with research using confidence is reviewed and new studies are presented. Two new studies asked misinformed subjects to provide reasons for their answers. In each study 15% to 27% of subjects said they remembered seeing items they had only read about. In another study subjects were asked to identify the source of misleading items they had seen in slides or read in misleading questions. Subjects were more likely to say they had seen in slides something they read about in the questions than they were to confuse information from two nearly identical sets of slides. Recent work shows that, not only is it possible to distort memory for events, it is possible to implant an entire memory for something that never happened. The evidence is now clear that we can become mentally tricked into making large as well as small changes in the way we recall the past. 相似文献