首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   203106篇
  免费   8813篇
  国内免费   157篇
  2020年   2834篇
  2019年   3487篇
  2018年   3998篇
  2017年   4335篇
  2016年   4945篇
  2015年   3963篇
  2014年   4835篇
  2013年   23545篇
  2012年   5668篇
  2011年   5147篇
  2010年   4408篇
  2009年   5036篇
  2008年   5080篇
  2007年   4653篇
  2006年   4921篇
  2005年   4744篇
  2004年   4320篇
  2003年   3884篇
  2002年   3679篇
  2001年   4913篇
  2000年   4611篇
  1999年   4104篇
  1998年   2970篇
  1997年   2772篇
  1996年   2695篇
  1995年   2518篇
  1994年   2478篇
  1993年   2429篇
  1992年   3383篇
  1991年   3188篇
  1990年   3069篇
  1989年   2926篇
  1988年   2868篇
  1987年   2802篇
  1986年   2845篇
  1985年   3065篇
  1984年   2946篇
  1983年   2659篇
  1982年   2493篇
  1981年   2455篇
  1980年   2287篇
  1979年   2712篇
  1978年   2444篇
  1977年   2343篇
  1976年   2194篇
  1975年   2441篇
  1974年   2589篇
  1973年   2487篇
  1972年   2054篇
  1971年   1957篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 62 毫秒
201.
Human communication is thoroughly context bound. We present two experiments investigating the importance of the shared context, that is, the amount of knowledge two interlocutors have in common, for the successful emergence and use of novel conventions. Using a referential communication task where black‐and‐white pictorial symbols are used to convey colors, pairs of participants build shared conventions peculiar to their dyad without experimenter feedback, relying purely on ostensive‐inferential communication. Both experiments demonstrate that access to the visual context promotes more successful communication. Importantly, success improves cumulatively, supporting the view that pairs establish conventional ways of using the symbols to communicate. Furthermore, Experiment 2 suggests that dyads with access to the visual context successfully adapt the conventions built for one color space to another color space, unlike dyads lacking it. In linking experimental pragmatics with language evolution, the study illustrates the benefits of exploring the emergence of linguistic conventions using an ostensive‐inferential model of communication.  相似文献   
202.
203.
Do the conditions under which promises are made determine whether they ought to be kept? Philosophers have placed a number of conditions on promising which, they hold, must be met in order to make promise-keeping obligatory. In so doing, they have distinguished valid promises from invalid promises and justified promises from promises that are not justified. Considering such conditions, one by one, we argue that they are mistaken. In the first place, the conditions they lay down are not necessary for either valid or justified promise-making. In the second place, promises need not meet such conditions in order to create moral obligations. In general, such analyses of promising fail because they suffer from a confusion between promise-making and promise-keeping. Philosophers have wrongly supposed that obligations to keep promises are dependent upon, or derivable from, the quality of the promises themselves, at the time they are made, instead of focusing on conditions that must be satisfied at the time when promises are supposed to be kept. It is not the quality of a promise that determines an obligation to keep it but the rightness or wrongness of performing the promised act.  相似文献   
204.
Ohne ZusammenfassungIn dankbarer Erinnerung an meinen verehrten Münchener Lehrer Kurt Huber, dessen frühzeitigen gewaltsamen Tod (13. 7. 43) wir noch immer beklagen. Die Arbeit entsprang einer seiner letzten wissenschaftlichen Anregungen.  相似文献   
205.
206.
Wray  K. Brad 《Synthese》2019,196(3):819-832
Synthese - Hoyningen-Huene (Systematicity: the nature of science, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2013) develops an account of what science is, distinguishing it from common sense. According to...  相似文献   
207.
208.
209.
210.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号