全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1487篇 |
免费 | 32篇 |
专业分类
1519篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 25篇 |
2019年 | 47篇 |
2018年 | 35篇 |
2017年 | 41篇 |
2016年 | 46篇 |
2015年 | 26篇 |
2014年 | 31篇 |
2013年 | 168篇 |
2012年 | 52篇 |
2011年 | 76篇 |
2010年 | 52篇 |
2009年 | 36篇 |
2008年 | 65篇 |
2007年 | 58篇 |
2006年 | 70篇 |
2005年 | 50篇 |
2004年 | 44篇 |
2003年 | 46篇 |
2002年 | 47篇 |
2001年 | 20篇 |
2000年 | 19篇 |
1999年 | 23篇 |
1998年 | 30篇 |
1997年 | 10篇 |
1996年 | 28篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 21篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 12篇 |
1986年 | 16篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 17篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 15篇 |
1977年 | 9篇 |
1976年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1973年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有1519条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
821.
This study explored friendship variables in relation to body image, dietary restraint, extreme weight-loss behaviors (EWEBs), and binge eating in adolescent girls. From 523 girls, 79 friendship cliques were identified using social network analysis. Participants completed questionnaires that assessed body image concerns, eating, friendship relations, and psychological family, and media variables. Similarity was greater for within than for between friendship cliques for body image concerns, dietary restraint, and EWLBs, but not for binge eating. Cliques high in body image concerns and dieting manifested these concerns in ways consistent with a high weight/shape-preoccupied subculture. Friendship attitudes contributed significantly to the prediction of individual body image concern and eating behaviors. Use of EWLBs by friends predicted an individual's own level of use. 相似文献
822.
823.
Text comprehension processes were investigated in children with hydrocephalus, a neurodevelopmental disorder associated with good word decoding, but deficient reading comprehension. In Experiment 1, hydrocephalus and control groups were similar in processes related to activating word meanings and using context to enhance meaning. The hydrocephalus group was poorer at suppressing contextually irrelevant meanings. In Experiment 2, the hydrocephalus group had difficulty integrating information from an earlier read sentence to understand a new sentence as textual distance between the two propositions increased, suggesting difficulty in reactivation processes related to comprehension. Results are discussed in relation to cognitive and neurocognitive models of comprehension. 相似文献
824.
In two experiments, we investigated the role of perceptual information in spurious recognition judgments. Participants viewed
lists of words in various unusual fonts. The frequency with which each font was presented was manipulated at study: Each font
was presented with 1 or 12 different words in Experiment 1 and with 1 or 20 words in Experiment 2. Although the participants
were instructed in a word recognition test to judge only on the basis of the word, regardless of font, there were significantly
more false alarms for new words seen in a previously presented font than for new words presented in a novel (not seen at study)
font in Experiment 1. In Experiment 2, the participants were significantly more likely to make a false alarm to a new word
seen in a font that had been used to present 20 words during study than to a font that had been used to present only 1 word
during study. The data show a mirror effect, in which words tested in low-frequency fonts produced more hits and fewer false
alarms than did words tested in high-frequency fonts. These results show that irrelevant perceptual information plays a role
in recognition judgments by providing spurious sources of familiarity and, thus, provide evidence that perceptual information
is represented and processed in the same way as semantic information. 相似文献
825.
Fox MD Allee MR Taylor GJ 《The American journal of bioethics : AJOB》2004,4(4):15-6; discussion W35-7
826.
Saigh PA Yasik AE Oberfield RA Halamandaris PV McHugh M 《Journal of abnormal psychology》2002,111(3):462-470
To test the differential validity of the posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) classification, 3 groups of youths (PTSD, traumatized PTSD negatives, and controls) were examined. Youth with major comorbid disorders were excluded. On the basis of an analysis of parent-derived Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) ratings, significant variations in CBCL scores were associated with PTSD but not with exposure to exceptional stress in the absence of PTSD. The results also indicated that traumatic exposure without the development of PTSD was not associated with higher estimates of psychopathology. 相似文献
827.
What are the best quantitative methods for studying cognitive decline? This question was investigated in a sample of 638 individuals aged 50 years and older from the Swedish Adoption/Twin Study of Aging. A battery of cognitive tests tapping multiple domains was administered to each individual from 2 to 7 times over a span of 10 years. Four methods of operationalizing cognitive decline were compared: change scores, a criterion-based method, least squares, and random effects regression (RER). The RER results were most consistent with a significant decline across measures and differences between demented and nondemented individuals. Predicted slopes from the RER model also showed the strongest interrelationships within and across cognitive domains as indicated by factor analysis results and stronger associations with demographic, health, and psychosocial predictors. 相似文献
828.
Pastoral Psychology - 相似文献
829.
A short-term longitudinal study was carried out on a group of 67 preschool children. At three points in time over a 12-month period, the children were given tests measuring their syllable, rime, and phoneme awareness, speech and language skills, and letter knowledge. In general, children's rime skills developed earlier than their phoneme skills. Structural equation models showed that articulatory skills and syllable and rime awareness predicted later phoneme awareness. 相似文献
830.