全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1325篇 |
免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
1341篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 8篇 |
2023年 | 9篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 26篇 |
2019年 | 29篇 |
2018年 | 36篇 |
2017年 | 32篇 |
2016年 | 38篇 |
2015年 | 35篇 |
2014年 | 23篇 |
2013年 | 154篇 |
2012年 | 73篇 |
2011年 | 64篇 |
2010年 | 58篇 |
2009年 | 37篇 |
2008年 | 63篇 |
2007年 | 60篇 |
2006年 | 63篇 |
2005年 | 46篇 |
2004年 | 58篇 |
2003年 | 43篇 |
2002年 | 53篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 13篇 |
1999年 | 21篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 11篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 13篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 14篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 12篇 |
1987年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 14篇 |
1985年 | 15篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 7篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有1341条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
881.
Based on 20 years of research on the social regulation of academic performances, this paper provides arguments for the idea
that the social context in which cognitive functioning takes place is an integral part of that functioning, not just the surrounding
context for it. Several studies in the classroom setting indeed reveal that student's cognitive performances can depend on
interactions between their actual and past social experiences, most notably interpersonal comparison and evaluation episodes.
Because of their recurrence, these episodes can become part of student's autobiographical memory and, therefore, can affect
their actual behavior in specific conditions. Theoretical suggestions are made for a ‘social psychology of cognition.’
This revised version was published online in August 2006 with corrections to the Cover Date. 相似文献
882.
In a picture-word interference task, picture naming is interfered by an incongruent word, but word naming is hardly hindered by the presence of an incongruent picture. In this study, we investigated whether Arabic digits are processed more like pictures or like words. We report two experiments in which Arabic digits and verbal numerals were confronted in a Stroop task. Arabic digit naming is interfered by the presence of an incongruent verbal numeral, while naming the verbal numeral is not influenced by the presence of an incongruent Arabic digit. In a second experiment, we excluded the hypothesis that the results are due to ignoring the Arabic digits: interferences from an incongruent distracter were similar for both notations in a semantic classification task. It seems that an asemantic conversion for Arabic digits is too slow to influence naming times, and that Arabic digit naming, like picture naming, is semantically mediated. 相似文献
883.
Expert and intermediate chess players attempted to choose the best move in five chess positions while their eye movements were monitored. Experts were faster and more accurate than intermediates in choosing the best move. Experts made fewer fixations per trial and greater amplitude saccades than did intermediates, but there was no difference in fixation duration across skill groups. Examining the spatial distribution of the first five fixations for each position by skill group revealed that experts produced more fixations on empty squares than did intermediates. When fixating pieces, experts produced a greater proportion of fixations on relevant pieces than did intermediates. It is argued that expert chess players perceptually encode chess configurations, rather than individual pieces, and, consequently, parafoveal or peripheral processing guides their eye movements, producing a pattern of saccadic selectivity by piece saliency. 相似文献
884.
W. James Greville Adam A. Cassar Mark K. Johansen Marc J. Buehner 《Memory & cognition》2013,41(6):904-916
Many studies have demonstrated that reinforcement delays exert a detrimental influence on human judgments of causality. In a free-operant procedure, the trial structure is usually only implicit, and delays are typically manipulated via trial duration, with longer trials tending to produce both longer experienced delays and also lower objective contingencies. If, however, a learner can become aware of this trial structure, this may mitigate the effects of delay on causal judgments. Here we tested this “structural-awareness” hypothesis by manipulating whether response–outcome contingencies were clearly identifiable as such, providing structural information in real time using an auditory tone to delineate consecutive trials. A first experiment demonstrated that providing cues to indicate trial structure, but without an explicit indication of their meaning, significantly increased the accuracy of causal judgments in the presence of delays. This effect was not mediated by changes in response frequency or timing, and a second experiment demonstrated that it cannot be attributed to the alternative explanation of enhanced outcome salience. In a third experiment, making trial structure explicit and unambiguous, by telling participants that the tones indicated trial structure, completely abolished the effect of delays. We concluded that, with sufficient information, a continuous stream of causes and effects can be perceived as a series of discrete trials, the contingency nature of the input may be exploited, and the effects of delay may be eliminated. These results have important implications for human contingency learning and in the characterization of temporal influences on causal inference. 相似文献
885.
Lindsay R. Duncan Michelle C. Bertoli Amy E. Latimer-Cheung Susan E. Rivers Marc A. Brackett Peter Salovey 《Personality and individual differences》2013
The purpose of this study was to test perceptions of the social consequences of smoking as a mediator of the relationship between emotional intelligence (EI) and intentions to smoke cigarettes among youth. Upper elementary school students (N = 255, Mage = 10.9 years, 49% male) completed measures of EI, verbal intelligence, smoking-related intentions, past behavior, and perceived social consequences. Mediation was tested using the Sobel test. Perceived social consequences was a marginally significant mediator of the impact of total EI on intentions to smoke (Z = 1.91, p = .057). Follow-up analyses showed that perceived social consequences significantly mediated the effect of 2 EI abilities on smoking intentions: using emotions (Z = 2.02, p = .043) and managing emotions (Z = 2.28, p = .023). The results indicate that the role of higher EI in deterring smoking among youth may be related to social processing ability (i.e., greater perceptions of the negative social consequences of smoking). These findings contribute to a growing body of research demonstrating that EI may reduce cigarette smoking among youth by influencing their social perceptions. 相似文献
886.
It is commonly accepted that social dominance orientation (SDO) and right‐wing authoritarianism (RWA) are potent unique predictors of a variety of prejudice and prejudice‐related constructs. However, contrary to some predictions, there has been little evidence that these constructs interact to produce this outcome—they appear to be additive but not interactive in their prediction of prejudice. We extend the interaction hypothesis to consideration of another broadly relevant construct—political ideology. Drawing from 14 independent New Zealand–based samples, we show, through meta‐analysis and multilevel random coefficient modelling, that SDO and RWA additively and interactively predict levels of political conservatism operationalised in a variety of ways. Specifically, both constructs are associated with increasing political conservatism, and the lowest levels of conservatism (or highest levels of political liberalism) are found in those lowest in both SDO and RWA. 相似文献
887.
888.
Pinkus RT Lockwood P Schimmack U Fournier MA 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2008,95(5):1180-1201
The authors examined the frequency, direction, and impact of social comparisons between romantic partners. Comparisons were expected to occur on a daily basis, owing to regular interactions between partners. To the extent that one empathizes and shares outcomes with one's partner, one might respond more positively to upward than to downward comparisons. Study 1a was an experience-sampling study in which participants reported comparisons made to their spouse over 2 weeks. Study 1b examined reactions to the most significant comparisons made during the experience-sampling study. Participants reported making comparisons to their romantic partner more than once a day on average and experienced more positive responses to upward than to downward comparisons. Study 2 demonstrated that participants empathized and shared outcomes with their partner to a greater extent than with a friend. Study 3 confirmed that participants responded more positively to upward than to downward comparisons even for domains high in self-relevance and even when the comparison had negative self-evaluative implications. These results suggest that, owing to higher levels of empathy and shared fate with partners, comparisons function differently in romantic than in other relationships. 相似文献
889.
Glenberg AM Sato M Cattaneo L Riggio L Palumbo D Buccino G 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》2008,61(6):905-919
Embodiment theory proposes that neural systems for perception and action are also engaged during language comprehension. Previous neuroimaging and neurophysiological studies have only been able to demonstrate modulation of action systems during comprehension of concrete language. We provide neurophysiological evidence for modulation of motor system activity during the comprehension of both concrete and abstract language. In Experiment 1, when the described direction of object transfer or information transfer (e.g., away from the reader to another) matched the literal direction of a hand movement used to make a response, speed of responding was faster than when the two directions mismatched (an action-sentence compatibility effect). In Experiment 2, we used single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation to study changes in the corticospinal motor pathways to hand muscles while reading the same sentences. Relative to sentences that do not describe transfer, there is greater modulation of activity in the hand muscles when reading sentences describing transfer of both concrete objects and abstract information. These findings are discussed in relation to the human mirror neuron system. 相似文献
890.
Framing effects are considered in a conversational framework using the well-known Asian Disease problem [Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453–458]. Speakers’ preferred message framing is examined and its corresponding persuasiveness is assessed using listeners’ responses. The results show that speakers exhibit a marked and consistent preference for positive over negative framing (Experiment 1). Judged from listeners’ responses, this preference is effective for promoting riskless, but not risky options. The incompatibility between speakers and listeners may be resolved by noting that speakers can jointly (i.e., comparatively) assess the information and the persuasive qualities of alternative frames. In contrast, listeners are exposed only to one of these frames and, consequently, can only assess the information separately (i.e., non-comparatively). Experiments 2 and 3 demonstrate that no incompatibility exists when both speakers and listeners are either in separate, or in joint evaluation mode. Differences between risky choice and attribute framing [Levin, I.P., Schneider, S.L., & Gaeth, G.J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: a typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76, 149–188] are briefly discussed. 相似文献