全文获取类型
收费全文 | 34839篇 |
免费 | 1542篇 |
国内免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
36407篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 410篇 |
2019年 | 540篇 |
2018年 | 775篇 |
2017年 | 744篇 |
2016年 | 825篇 |
2015年 | 543篇 |
2014年 | 657篇 |
2013年 | 2870篇 |
2012年 | 1245篇 |
2011年 | 1247篇 |
2010年 | 715篇 |
2009年 | 798篇 |
2008年 | 1073篇 |
2007年 | 1070篇 |
2006年 | 929篇 |
2005年 | 814篇 |
2004年 | 800篇 |
2003年 | 733篇 |
2002年 | 748篇 |
2001年 | 1088篇 |
2000年 | 1097篇 |
1999年 | 817篇 |
1998年 | 367篇 |
1997年 | 337篇 |
1996年 | 325篇 |
1995年 | 340篇 |
1992年 | 639篇 |
1991年 | 595篇 |
1990年 | 571篇 |
1989年 | 551篇 |
1988年 | 541篇 |
1987年 | 488篇 |
1986年 | 487篇 |
1985年 | 537篇 |
1984年 | 440篇 |
1983年 | 404篇 |
1982年 | 315篇 |
1979年 | 476篇 |
1978年 | 330篇 |
1976年 | 319篇 |
1975年 | 362篇 |
1974年 | 435篇 |
1973年 | 462篇 |
1972年 | 399篇 |
1971年 | 343篇 |
1970年 | 317篇 |
1969年 | 334篇 |
1968年 | 416篇 |
1967年 | 370篇 |
1966年 | 357篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Two experiments investigated the effects of successive reinforcement contexts on choice. In the first, concurrent variable-interval schedules of primary reinforcement operated during the initial links of concurrent chains. The rate of this reinforcement arranged by the concurrent schedules was decreased across conditions: When it was higher than the terminal-link rate, preference for the higher frequency initial-link schedule increased relative to baseline. (During baseline, a standard concurrent-schedule procedure was in effect). When the initial-link reinforcement rate was lower than the terminal-link rate, preference converged toward indifference. In the second experiment, a chain schedule was available on a third key while a concurrent schedule was in effect on the side keys. When the terminal link of the chain schedule was produced, the side keys became inoperative. Availability of the chain schedule did not affect choice between the concurrent schedules. These results show that only when successive reinforcement contexts are produced by choice responding do those successive contexts affect choice in concurrent schedules. 相似文献
102.
Typical delay determines waiting time on periodic-food schedules: Static and dynamic tests 总被引:6,自引:6,他引:0 下载免费PDF全文
Pigeons and other animals soon learn to wait (pause) after food delivery on periodic-food schedules before resuming the food-rewarded response. Under most conditions the steady-state duration of the average waiting time, t, is a linear function of the typical interfood interval. We describe three experiments designed to explore the limits of this process. In all experiments, t was associated with one key color and the subsequent food delay, T, with another. In the first experiment, we compared the relation between t (waiting time) and T (food delay) under two conditions: when T was held constant, and when T was an inverse function of t. The pigeons could maximize the rate of food delivery under the first condition by setting t to a consistently short value; optimal behavior under the second condition required a linear relation with unit slope between t and T. Despite this difference in optimal policy, the pigeons in both cases showed the same linear relation, with slope less than one, between t and T. This result was confirmed in a second parametric experiment that added a third condition, in which T + t was held constant. Linear waiting appears to be an obligatory rule for pigeons. In a third experiment we arranged for a multiplicative relation between t and T (positive feedback), and produced either very short or very long waiting times as predicted by a quasi-dynamic model in which waiting time is strongly determined by the just-preceding food delay. 相似文献
103.
Jeffrey S. Levin Ph.D. M.P.H. C. David Jenkins Ph.D. Robert M. Rose M. D. 《Journal of religion and health》1988,27(4):267-278
In a study of air traffic controllers, religious differences are found in the way Type A behavior is associated with several health status indicators. Associations between the Jenkins Activity Survey (JAS) and physical illness incidence, health-promotive behavior, diastolic and systolic blood pressure, subjective distress and impulse control problems, and alcohol consumption are examined by religious attendance, religious affiliation, and change in affiliation. Findings confirm that Type A does not vary significantly by religion. However, there are several significant findings between Type A and various health indicators. Type A is associated with illness incidence, overall and more strongly in several religion, subgroups. Type A and alcohol consumption are related positively in Protestants and converts, and negatively in churchgoing Catholics. Type A is related to impulse control problems in churchgoing Protestants and to subjective distress in churchgoing Catholics. Finally, in individuals with weak or no religious ties, Type A is associated with lower blood pressure. This last finding suggests that in some people (for example, the irreligious or unchurched), the coronary-prone behavior pattern may have cardiovascular effects which are salutary in at least one respect.The authors wish to thank Dr. Kyriakos S. Markides and Laura A. Ray for their assistance with this study. Address requests for reprints to Dr. Levin at the Institute of Gerontology, 300 North Ingalls, Ann Arbor, MI 48109. 相似文献
104.
105.
Jeffrey C. King 《Philosophical Studies》1988,53(3):417-440
I have benefited greatly from discussing the issues addressed in this paper with Michael Liston, John Vickers and Mark Wilson. 相似文献
106.
The purpose of this study was to investigate differences in scores of 59 mentally retarded adults (mild, moderate, severe, or profound), 133 advantaged, and 130 disadvantaged (low income) children in Grades 1, 2, 3 on the Primary Measures of Music Audiation. Test-retest reliabilities for the Tonal and Rhythm subtests were .81 and .86, respectively, for the retarded group. Analysis of variance indicated that the mildly retarded children performed significantly better than other groups of retarded children on these two tests. 2 x 3 analyses of variance indicated that third graders from both advantaged and disadvantaged groups performed significantly better than the first or second graders on both subtests and that advantaged children performed significantly better than the disadvantaged. However, disadvantaged children made greater gains, especially for third graders, than the advantaged. 相似文献
107.
108.
109.
S-R compatibility between response position and destination of apparent motion: evidence of the detection of affordances 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
C F Michaels 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》1988,14(2):231-240
In choice reaction time, stimuli and responses in some combinations (e.g., based on spatial arrangement) are faster than in other combinations. To test whether motion toward a position yields faster responses at that position, a computer-generated square in front of one hand appeared to move either toward that hand or toward the other hand. Compatible responses (e.g., motion toward left hand/left response) were faster than incompatible responses, even when that opposed traditional positional compatibility. In Experiment 2, subjects responded to the same stimuli but with both hands left, right, or on the body midline. Medial responses were the fastest, showing that destination, rather than mere relative position, was a critical variable. It was suggested that spatial compatibility effects are not unique to position but apply to a variety of task situations, describable by J.J. Gibson's theory of affordances, in which he claims that one perceives the actions (e.g., catching) permitted in a situation. 相似文献
110.
Black and White college students were studied to determine reactions to moral dilemmas when the race of the central character in the dilemma was changed. 相似文献