首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3598篇
  免费   1412篇
  2021年   75篇
  2020年   81篇
  2019年   386篇
  2018年   271篇
  2017年   388篇
  2016年   375篇
  2015年   342篇
  2014年   300篇
  2013年   552篇
  2012年   272篇
  2011年   210篇
  2010年   249篇
  2009年   225篇
  2008年   200篇
  2007年   130篇
  2006年   109篇
  2005年   123篇
  2004年   96篇
  2003年   111篇
  2002年   110篇
  2001年   87篇
  2000年   76篇
  1999年   47篇
  1998年   14篇
  1997年   11篇
  1996年   4篇
  1995年   5篇
  1994年   5篇
  1993年   6篇
  1992年   10篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   13篇
  1989年   6篇
  1988年   4篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   7篇
  1985年   8篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   4篇
  1981年   5篇
  1979年   4篇
  1978年   7篇
  1977年   4篇
  1976年   4篇
  1974年   4篇
  1973年   8篇
  1972年   10篇
  1971年   4篇
  1967年   5篇
  1955年   3篇
排序方式: 共有5010条查询结果,搜索用时 13 毫秒
991.
This study examined which body part labels children could (i) produce when the experimenter touched different locations on her own body, asking each time ‘What's this?’ and (ii) comprehend by touching the correct locations on their own bodies in response to the experimenter asking ‘Where's the [body‐part label]?’. Seventeen children aged between 26 and 41 months, tested in a repeated measures procedure, were presented with 50 different body part stimuli in 200 test trials per child. Overall, the children produced fewer body part labels than they could comprehend. The accuracy of children's responses depended on (i) the location or extent of each body part (facial and broad body features were better known; joints and features in or attached to broad body parts the least well known); (ii) the amount of sensory (but not motor) representation each body part has in the human cortex; and (iii) whether a body part was commonly named by caregivers. These results present a precise mapping of the body parts that young children are able to name and locate on their own bodies in response to body part names; they suggest several possible determinants of lexical‐semantic body knowledge and add to the understanding of how it develops in childhood.  相似文献   
992.
993.
994.
This investigation dealt with the role and impact of affect in the process of resistance. A total of 597 participants took part in the study in 4 phases spanning 6 weeks. Initial results indicated that the cognitive, affective‐anger, and affective‐happiness inoculation treatments all conferred resistance to persuasive attacks. Structural equation analyses were conducted on the cognitive, affective‐anger, and affective‐happiness experimental inoculation conditions in order to examine the process of resistance. The results across all 3 conditions revealed a direct path in which inoculation treatments directly induced resistance to persuasive attacks. However, indirect paths to resistance varied across the 3 experimental conditions. Cognitive inoculation treatments contributed to receiver threat and counterarguing output, which, in turn, enhanced resistance. Thus, the cognitive inoculation treatments triggered a process that is consistent with McGuire's theoretical explanation for resistance. By contrast, both affective‐anger and affective‐happiness inoculation treatments relied more heavily on elicited emotional responses. Finally, the results indicated that greater receiver involvement was positively associated with experienced anger and, therefore, indirectly contributed to resistance, whereas greater receiver self‐efficacy tended to dampen resistance.  相似文献   
995.
996.
997.
998.
999.
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号