全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4207篇 |
免费 | 68篇 |
专业分类
4275篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 40篇 |
2020年 | 76篇 |
2019年 | 62篇 |
2018年 | 124篇 |
2017年 | 109篇 |
2016年 | 105篇 |
2015年 | 79篇 |
2014年 | 73篇 |
2013年 | 329篇 |
2012年 | 140篇 |
2011年 | 124篇 |
2010年 | 79篇 |
2009年 | 93篇 |
2008年 | 104篇 |
2007年 | 114篇 |
2006年 | 90篇 |
2005年 | 85篇 |
2004年 | 75篇 |
2003年 | 62篇 |
2002年 | 56篇 |
2001年 | 128篇 |
2000年 | 91篇 |
1999年 | 79篇 |
1996年 | 39篇 |
1993年 | 43篇 |
1992年 | 60篇 |
1991年 | 70篇 |
1990年 | 59篇 |
1989年 | 56篇 |
1988年 | 63篇 |
1987年 | 57篇 |
1986年 | 50篇 |
1985年 | 59篇 |
1984年 | 60篇 |
1983年 | 46篇 |
1982年 | 51篇 |
1981年 | 38篇 |
1979年 | 57篇 |
1978年 | 57篇 |
1977年 | 51篇 |
1976年 | 45篇 |
1975年 | 64篇 |
1974年 | 61篇 |
1973年 | 56篇 |
1972年 | 52篇 |
1971年 | 52篇 |
1969年 | 37篇 |
1968年 | 49篇 |
1967年 | 60篇 |
1966年 | 43篇 |
排序方式: 共有4275条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
201.
The possibility that rats can navigate in the Morris water maze by reducing the difference between the memorized platform scene and the current sensory input was tested in nine blind rats. A computerized videosystem monitored the rats' movements in the pool and converted the rat-target distance into tones the frequency of which increased in 64 equal steps from 120 Hz at 128 cm to 7680 Hz at 0 cm. During 15 days of training to find a fixed platform position from different starting points (12 trials per day) average escape latencies decreased from 39.0 to 25.4 s. The performance significantly deteriorated when the acoustic distance signalization was omitted and/or when the target position was changed form trial to trial. It is concluded that blind rats solved the task by simultaneously employing search strategy based on position responses, mapping using acoustic background beacons, and distance reduction navigation. It is argued that the various strategies are additive and that their relative significance depends of the conditions of the experiment. 相似文献
202.
203.
AMELIE O. RORTY 《Midwest Studies In Philosophy》1988,13(1):136-148
204.
The manipulation of stimulus significance, by instructions from the experimenter, may be taken as an example of verbal conditioning. Consideration of such a mechanism suggested that personality effects previously found in conditioning studies should be apparent in instructional manipulations of significance in a study of the orienting response (OR) to words. Because of recent changes in dimensioning of the personality structure, some of the items originally used to define Eysenck’s extraversion (E) dimension are now used to assess the new dimension of psychoticism (P), suggesting that at least some of the established effects of E upon conditioning may be associated now with P. Hence the P scale was focused on in this study. Words differing on the evaluative dimension of the semantic differential were presented in three blocks, the first under indifferent instructions, the second under instructions to rate the words for their affective impact, and the third under indifferent instructions again. These blocks correspond to baseline, conditioning, and extinction conditions respectively. Electrodermal activity indicated enhanced conditioning, together with greater carry-over effects in the extinction phase, for low-P compared with high-P subjects. The results indicate the importance of personality effects in studies of stimulus significance and illustrate the value of the verbal conditioning mechanism in this area of the OR field. They also suggest the need to re-examine previously obtained E-effects in conditioning studies in light of changing personality tests. 相似文献
205.
Meta-awareness of bias in intimate partner judgments was investigated in 3 studies. In Study 1, participants rated fictional partners in happier relationships as more positively biased in their partner perceptions. In Study 2, participants thought their judgments of their own current partners were positively biased and that they were judged by their partners in a positively biased fashion. Using a sample of couples, Study 3 showed that metaperceptions of bias were anchored to actual levels of bias at the individual and relationship levels. In addition, positive bias was accentuated for traits that were more relevant to mate evaluation. These findings (as expected) suggest that positive bias in partner judgments can be a normative and consciously accessible feature of intimate relationships. 相似文献
206.
This paper studies the reliability and validity of naturalistic speech errors as a tool for language production research. Possible biases when collecting naturalistic speech errors are identified and specific predictions derived. These patterns are then contrasted with published reports from Germanic languages (English, German and Dutch) and one Romance language (Spanish). Unlike findings in the Germanic languages, Spanish speech errors show many patterns which run contrary to those expected from bias: (1) more phonological errors occur between words than within word; (2) word-initial consonants are less likely to participate in errors than word-medial consonants, (3) errors are equally likely in stressed and in unstressed syllables, (4) perseverations are more frequent than anticipations, and (5) there is no trace of a lexical bias. We present a new corpus of Spanish speech errors collected by many theoretically naïve observers (whereas the only corpus available so far was collected by two highly trained theoretically informed observers), give a general overview of it, and use it to replicate previous reports. In spite of the different susceptibility of these methods to bias, results were remarkably similar in both corpora and again contrary to predictions from bias. As a result, collecting speech errors “in the wild” seems to be free of bias to a reasonable extent even when using a multiple-collector method. The observed contrasting patterns between Spanish and Germanic languages arise as true cross-linguistic differences. 相似文献
207.
Scott O. Lilienfeld 《Perspectives on Psychological Science》2007,2(1):53-70
ABSTRACT— The phrase primum non nocere ("first, do no harm") is a well‐accepted credo of the medical and mental health professions. Although emerging data indicate that several psychological treatments may produce harm in significant numbers of individuals, psychologists have until recently paid little attention to the problem of hazardous treatments. I critically evaluate and update earlier conclusions regarding deterioration effects in psychotherapy, outline methodological obstacles standing in the way of identifying potentially harmful therapies (PHTs), provide a provisional list of PHTs, discuss the implications of PHTs for clinical science and practice, and delineate fruitful areas for further research on PHTs. A heightened emphasis on PHTs should narrow the scientist–practitioner gap and safeguard mental health consumers against harm. Moreover, the literature on PHTs may provide insight into underlying mechanisms of change that cut across many domains of psychotherapy. The field of psychology should prioritize its efforts toward identifying PHTs and place greater emphasis on potentially dangerous than on empirically supported therapies. 相似文献
208.
Anxious individuals selectively attend to threatening information, and defensiveness may influence the experience and expression
of anxiety. Fifty-eight undergraduates scoring high and low on measures of anxiety and defensiveness viewed pleasant, neutral,
and unpleasant pictures. Acoustic startle probes were presented 60, 240, or 2,000 ms after picture onset. At 60 ms and 240 ms,
repressors (low anxiety, high defensiveness) showed weaker blinks during both pleasant and unpleasant compared to neutral
pictures, suggesting enhanced early attention to affective stimuli, regardless of valence. At 2,000 ms, high-anxious but not
low anxious participants showed potentiated startle responses during unpleasant compared to pleasant pictures. Although this
result replicated previous research on anxiety and valence modulation of startle, exploratory analyses suggested that the
valence effect was restricted to trait anxious individuals tending toward a defensive coping style. Across probe conditions,
defensiveness was associated with heightened startle reactivity independent of self-reported anxiety and foreground stimulus
characteristics.
相似文献
Richard O. TempleEmail: |
209.
This study examined the specificity of disgust sensitivity in predicting contamination-related anxiety and behavioral avoidance. Participants high (n=26) and low (n=30) in contamination fear completed self-report measures of disgust sensitivity, contamination cognitions (overestimation of the likelihood and severity of contamination from everyday objects), anxiety, and depression. They then completed three randomly presented contamination-based behavioral avoidance tasks (BATs) that consisted of exposure to a used comb, a cookie on the floor, and a bedpan filled with toilet water. Results indicated that disgust sensitivity was significantly associated with anxious and avoidant responding to the contamination-related BATs. This association remained largely intact after controlling for gender, contamination fear group membership, anxiety, and depression. Contamination cognitions were also significantly related to BAT responses. However, this relationship was fully mediated by disgust sensitivity. These findings indicate that disgust sensitivity has a specific and robust association with contamination concerns commonly observed in obsessive compulsive disorder. The findings are discussed in the context of a disease-avoidance model. 相似文献
210.
Hung-Yi Lu Donald O Case Mia Liza A Lustria Nahyun Kwon James E Andrews Sarah E Cavendish Brenikki R Floyd 《Cyberpsychology & behavior》2007,10(5):709-712
This study explores factors influencing international students' likelihood of using the Internet to seek disaster-related information should a disaster affect their countries. A cross-sectional survey was conducted in two universities in America between August 1 and September 30, 2005. Two hundred twenty-nine (n = 229) students completed the self-administered questionnaires. ANOVA analyses found that respondents' Internet self-efficacy had no significant impact on their intentions to seek disaster-related information on the Internet. However, respondents' Internet dependency and attitude toward seeking information online were found to have a significant effect on such intentions. 相似文献