全文获取类型
收费全文 | 105篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
108篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 1篇 |
2019年 | 1篇 |
2018年 | 1篇 |
2017年 | 1篇 |
2016年 | 1篇 |
2014年 | 3篇 |
2013年 | 14篇 |
2012年 | 7篇 |
2011年 | 3篇 |
2010年 | 2篇 |
2008年 | 8篇 |
2007年 | 3篇 |
2006年 | 6篇 |
2005年 | 3篇 |
2004年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 1篇 |
1997年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 1篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 4篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 4篇 |
1969年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有108条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
Gambling is an activity that will become increasingly known to the counselling profession as the problematic effects of gambling deregulation in the United Kingdom slowly take effect over the nextfive to ten years. The gambling literature and the phases of the pathological gambler's career are briefly examined. Counselling approaches that have been used in the treatment of pathological gambling include (i) psychotherapy, (ii) conjoint marital therapy, (iii) minimal interventions, (iv) behavioural counselling, and (v) practical approaches to the treatment of adolescent problem gamblers. It is concluded that, although a number of evaluation studies have been carried out, few are directly comparable with one another, and several have methodological problems which may undermine the value of the evidence produced. 相似文献
42.
43.
44.
This work investigates production preferences in different languages. Specifically, it examines how animacy, competition processes, and language-specific constraints shape speakers' choices of structure. English, Spanish and Serbian speakers were presented with depicted events in which either an animate or inanimate entity was acted upon by an agent. Questions about the affected participant in these events prompted the production of relative clauses identifying these entities (e.g., the bag the woman is punching). Results indicated that in English, animacy plays a strong role in determining the choice of passive structures. In contrast, it plays a less prominent role in Spanish and Serbian structure choices, where more active structures were produced to varying degrees. Critically, the semantic similarity between the agent and the patient of the event correlated with the omission of the agent in all languages, indicating that competition resulted in the agent's inhibition. Similarity also correlated with different functional choices in Spanish. The results suggest that similarity-based competition may influence various stages of production planning but its manifestations are constrained by language-specific grammatical options. Implications for models of sentence production and the relationship between production and comprehension are discussed. 相似文献
45.
Cynthia Macdonald 《Synthese》2014,191(15):3685-3710
It is widely accepted that knowledge of certain of one’s own mental states is authoritative in being epistemically more secure than knowledge of the mental states of others, and theories of self-knowledge have largely appealed to one or the other of two sources to explain this special epistemic status. The first, ‘detectivist’, position, appeals to an inner perception-like basis, whereas the second, ‘constitutivist’, one, appeals to the view that the special security awarded to certain self-knowledge is a conceptual matter. I argue that there is a fundamental class of cases of authoritative self-knowledge, ones in which subjects are consciously thinking about their current, conscious intentional states, that is best accounted for in terms of a theory that is, broadly speaking, introspectionist and detectivist. The position developed has an intuitive plausibility that has inspired many who work in the Cartesian tradition, and the potential to yield a single treatment of the basis of authoritative self-knowledge for both intentional states and sensation states. 相似文献
46.
James S. P. Macdonald Patrick Cavanagh Rufin VanRullen 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2014,76(1):64-72
Attending to a periodic motion stimulus can induce illusory reversals of the direction of motion. This continuous wagon wheel illusion (c-WWI) has been taken to reflect discrete sampling of motion information by visual attention. An alternative view is that it is caused by adaptation. Here, we attempt to discriminate between these two interpretations by asking participants to attend to multiple periodic motion stimuli: The discrete attentional sampling account, but not the adaptation account, predicts a decrease of c-WWI temporal-frequency tuning with set size (with a single periodic motion stimulus the c-WWI is tuned to a temporal frequency of 10 Hz). We presented one to four rotating gratings that occasionally reversed direction while participants counted reversals. We considered reversal overestimations as manifestations of the c-WWI and determined the temporal-frequency tuning of the illusion for each set size. Optimal temporal frequency decreased with increasing set size. This outcome favors the discrete attentional sampling interpretation of the c-WWI, with a sampling rate for each individual stimulus dependent on the number of stimuli attended. 相似文献
47.
Dimitrios Adamis Adrian Treloar Finbarr C. Martin Alastair J. D. Macdonald 《Science and engineering ethics》2010,16(1):169-174
Here we describe how more important findings were obtained in a delirium study by using an informal assessment of mental capacity,
and, in those who lacked capacity, obtaining consent later when or if capacity returned or a proxy was found. From a total
of 233 patients 23 patients lacked capacity as judged by our informal capacity judgment and 210 did not. Of those who lacked
capacity, 13 agreed to enter in the study. Six of them regained capacity later. When these 13 participants were excluded from
analysis, significant findings were no longer evident. These results show that by the inclusion of subjects who lacked capacity
the results of analyses of the condition from whish they suffer are altered. We suggest that this approach to the study of
delirium is more ethical than the usual system of strict exclusion of people who lack capacity to give consent and for whom
assent is not available. 相似文献
48.
49.
Within this study we used self-report measures completed by 123 undergraduate students from an Australian university to investigate the validity of Peterson and Seligman’s [Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P (2004). Character strengths and virtues. New York: Oxford.] classification system of 24 character strengths and six virtues. We also looked at how the 24 character strengths relate to the Five Factor Model of personality and to a measure of social desirability. Using a second order factor analysis of the 24 character strengths, we found that these 24 character strengths did not produce a factor structure consistent with the six higher order virtues as proposed by Peterson and Seligman [Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P (2004). Character strengths and virtues. New York: Oxford.]. Instead, the 24 character strengths were well represented by both a one and four factor solution. Patterns of significant relationships between each of the 24 character strengths, the one and four factor solutions and the Five Factor Model of personality were found. The results have implications for [Peterson, C., & Seligman, M. E. P (2004). Character strengths and virtues. New York: Oxford.] classification. 相似文献
50.