全文获取类型
收费全文 | 230篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
231篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 2篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 4篇 |
2020年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 8篇 |
2017年 | 9篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 12篇 |
2013年 | 20篇 |
2012年 | 17篇 |
2011年 | 13篇 |
2010年 | 18篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 16篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 6篇 |
2004年 | 7篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 3篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有231条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
Charles H. Shea Qin Lai David L. Wright Maarten Immink Charles Black 《Journal of motor behavior》2013,45(2):139-152
The authors conducted the present experiments to resolve the discrepancy between studies in which relative-timing learning has been found to be enhanced by consistent practice conditions and contextual interference experiments in which relative-timing learning has been found to be enhanced more by random practice than by blocked practice. There were 40 participants in Experiment 1 and 48 in Experiment 2. The results of Experiment 1 extended previous findings: The learning of the relative-timing pattern was systematically enhanced by the degree to which the practice conditions promoted movement consistency (constant > blocked > serial > random). Experiment 2 provided evidence that the discrepancy between the relative-timing effects in the 2 groups of studies was a product of the way in which relative-timing goals and feedback were presented. When the feedback was presented as segment times, random practice resulted in generally more stable relative-timing patterns during acquisition than blocked practice did. Thus, in both experiments, the learning of the relative-timing pattern was enhanced by more stable relative-timing conditions during acquisition. Absolute-timing learning, as indexed by the transfer tests, was enhanced by serial or random practice as compared with constant or blocked practice, and was relatively unaffected by feedback conditions directed at the relative-timing pattern. In terms of motor programming theory, those findings are taken as additional evidence for the disassociation of memories supporting generalized motor program (GMP) performance, as indexed by relative timing, and parameter performance, as indexed by absolute timing. 相似文献
162.
163.
164.
165.
Sandra C. Matz Cristina Segalin David Stillwell Sandrine R. Müller Maarten W. Bos 《Journal of Consumer Psychology》2019,29(3):370-390
Images play a central role in digital marketing. They attract attention, trigger emotions, and shape consumers’ first impressions of products and brands. We propose that the shift from one‐to‐many mass communication to highly personalized one‐to‐one communication requires an understanding of image appeal at a personal level. Instead of asking “How appealing is this image?” we ask “How appealing is this image to this particular consumer?” Using the well‐established five‐factor model of personality, we apply machine learning algorithms to predict an image's personality appeal—the personality of consumers to which the image appeals most—from a set of 89 automatically extracted image features (Study 1). We subsequently apply the same algorithm on new images to predict consequential outcomes from the fit between consumer and image personality. We show that image‐person fit adds incremental predictive power over the images’ general appeal when predicting (a) consumers’ liking of new images (Study 2) and (b) consumers’ attitudes and purchase intentions (Study 3). 相似文献
166.
Maarten Marsman Eric-Jan Wagenmakers 《European Journal of Developmental Psychology》2017,14(5):545-555
We illustrate the Bayesian approach to data analysis using the newly developed statistical software program JASP. With JASP, researchers are able to take advantage of the benefits that the Bayesian framework has to offer in terms of parameter estimation and hypothesis testing. The Bayesian advantages are discussed using real data on the relation between Quality of Life and Executive Functioning in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. 相似文献
167.
Why do irrational beliefs adopt the trappings of science, to become what is known as “pseudoscience”? Here, we develop and extend an epidemiological framework to map the factors that explain the form and the popularity of irrational beliefs in scientific garb. These factors include the exploitation of epistemic vigilance, the misunderstanding of the authority of science, the use of the honorific title of “science” as an explicit argument for belief, and the phenomenon of epistemic negligence. We conclude by integrating the various factors in an epidemiological framework and thus provide a comprehensive cultural evolutionary account of science mimicry. 相似文献
168.
Maarten A. Hajer 《Knowledge, Technology, and Policy》1995,8(4):26-39
This article examines how “design rationality” could help remediate the controversy over environmental degradation. Drawing
on the case of designing sustainable forms of traffic management, it argues that this will only be effective to a limited
degree. “Policy conversation” does indeed take place but within a coalition of actions that pushes a particular set of solutions.
This facilitates due procedure but erodes political legitimacy, thus potentially reproducing an intractable controversy. The
article suggests a five-phase model of democratic control as an alternative.
He primarily works in the field of sociology of technology and environment. He is presently involved in a research project
on the social redefinition of mobility, analyzing the translation of sustainable development into new institutional arrangements. 相似文献
169.
For an arbitrary similarity type of Boolean Algebras with Operators we define a class ofSahlqvist identities. Sahlqvist identities have two important properties. First, a Sahlqvist identity is valid in a complex algebra if and only if the underlying relational atom structure satisfies a first-order condition which can be effectively read off from the syntactic form of the identity. Second, and as a consequence of the first property, Sahlqvist identities arecanonical, that is, their validity is preserved under taking canonical embedding algebras. Taken together, these properties imply that results about a Sahlqvist variety V van be obtained by reasoning in the elementary class of canonical structures of algebras in V.We give an example of this strategy in the variety of Cylindric Algebras: we show that an important identity calledHenkin's equation is equivalent to a simpler identity that uses only one variable. We give a conceptually simple proof by showing that the first-order correspondents of these two equations are equivalent over the class of cylindric atom structures.Presented byIstván Németi 相似文献
170.
Maarten Franssen 《Synthese》1994,101(2):249-272
Gauthier's argument for constrained maximization, presented inMorals by Agreement, is perfected by taking into account the possibility of accidental exploitation and discussing the limitations on the values of the parameters which measure the translucency of the actors. Gauthier's argument is nevertheless shown to be defective concerning the rationality of constrained maximization as a strategic choice. It can be argued that it applies only to a single actor entering a population of individuals who are themselves not rational actors but simple rule-followers. A proper analysis of the strategic choice situation involving two rational actors who confront each other shows that constrained maximization as the choice of both actors can only result under very demanding assumptions. 相似文献