全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3256篇 |
免费 | 113篇 |
国内免费 | 23篇 |
专业分类
3392篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 35篇 |
2023年 | 37篇 |
2022年 | 32篇 |
2021年 | 51篇 |
2020年 | 88篇 |
2019年 | 91篇 |
2018年 | 122篇 |
2017年 | 98篇 |
2016年 | 105篇 |
2015年 | 83篇 |
2014年 | 82篇 |
2013年 | 325篇 |
2012年 | 135篇 |
2011年 | 142篇 |
2010年 | 113篇 |
2009年 | 81篇 |
2008年 | 124篇 |
2007年 | 135篇 |
2006年 | 128篇 |
2005年 | 86篇 |
2004年 | 102篇 |
2003年 | 55篇 |
2002年 | 60篇 |
2001年 | 130篇 |
2000年 | 128篇 |
1999年 | 77篇 |
1998年 | 20篇 |
1997年 | 33篇 |
1996年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1992年 | 44篇 |
1991年 | 55篇 |
1990年 | 43篇 |
1989年 | 45篇 |
1988年 | 43篇 |
1987年 | 29篇 |
1986年 | 21篇 |
1985年 | 25篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 20篇 |
1979年 | 22篇 |
1976年 | 15篇 |
1974年 | 16篇 |
1973年 | 16篇 |
1971年 | 17篇 |
1970年 | 15篇 |
1969年 | 14篇 |
1968年 | 14篇 |
1967年 | 16篇 |
排序方式: 共有3392条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
11.
Prior discussion of hypothesizing as a session "guideline" has not fully clarified the derivation of hypotheses. Reviewing relevant philosophy of science literature, the authors develop the notions of evidence and evidence-gathering in systemic family therapy. The authors claim evidence-gathering and hypothesizing are interdependent, reciprocal elements of scientific understanding. The structure of evidence is described along with practical and qualitative rules for collecting and using evidence. This model is applied to a case example illustrating the logic of clinical process. Implications are discussed for family therapy training and supervision. 相似文献
12.
13.
14.
KJELL FLEKKØY 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》1987,28(1):69-74
The total number of different responses, given by a group of subjects to a stimulus word in a single-word, free-association situation ( D ), is a primary determinant of associative response latency (Tecce & Glassco, 1965; Flekkøy, 1981). D has traditionally been assumed to reflect a word's associative potential, and the increased latency to high D words has been explained by response competition. The results of the present experiments indicate, however, that D reflects in an inverse manner semantic autonomy , high D words requiring more contextual information for their semantic specification than low D words. The process of semantic specification requires time, creating the positive D -response latency relationship. Based on a semantic interpretation of D , associative response heterogeneity may be used as an index of a word's semantic autonomy. 相似文献
15.
Augustus Y. Napier PhD 《Contemporary Family Therapy》1987,9(1-2):42-57
This is the second of two papers dealing with symbolic-experiential treatment of marital issues. The first—Early Stages in Experiential Marital Therapy — discussed three early stages. This paper discusses regressive work with couples, including engagement with the family of origin. 相似文献
16.
A questionnaire monitoring occupational aspiration, ethnic identification, adolescent experience, and self-esteem was administered to a large sample of Indian and Anglo-Saxon British male and female adolescents attending school in the West Midlands. The relationship between these variables and differences between the four groups were consistent with predictions derived from the social identity approach to intergroup relations and group behaviour (Tajfel & Turner, 1979). Indian males were found to possess a social mobility belief structure that mediates high occupational aspirations and keeness to marry out of their ethnic group. In contrast, Indian females were found to possess a social change belief structure associated with acceptance of the status quo and lower aspirations. Males and Anglo-Saxons reported higher self-esteem than females or Indians. The adolescent experience findings were less clear, but were consistent with the general interpretation of the data that the differences in intersex relations and gender-related experiences are contingent on ethnicity. 相似文献
17.
18.
The present study investigates effects of different types of instructions (high-rate, interval, and minimal) during training with a fixed-interval schedule as a function of prior acquired temporal knowledge. A pretest was used to assess 4 1/2- and 6-year-old children's ability to understand the temporal parameters of a fixed-interval schedule of reinforcement. The results as a whole show that the control exerted by instructions given by the experimenter or elaborated by the subjects themselves on fixed-interval performance of young children depends on the interaction of two factors: development of verbal self-control skills and mastery of knowledge required by the rules forming the instructions. 相似文献
19.
The present study used a multitrait-multimethod technique [D.T. Campbell and D. W. Fiske (1959) “Convergent and Discriminant Validation by the Multitrait-Multimethod Matrix,” Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105] to examine Bem's [(1974) “The Measurement of Psychological Androgyny,” Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 152–162] use of an instrumental-expressive dichotomy to conceptualize the Masculinity and Femininity scales of the Bem Sex Role Inventory as statistically orthogonal constructs. Results indicate that while perhaps reliable, masculinity and femininity lack clear convergent and discriminant validity. Therefore, Bem's contention that masculinity and femininity are orthogonal constructs is questioned. Results are discussed in light of trait- vs. method-driven research. 相似文献
20.
K Hikami Y Hasegawa T Matsuzawa 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1990,104(3):233-237
In Experiment 1, 3 mother-child pairs of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) were given simultaneous choice tests between raisins and popcorn. The mothers and offspring showed different choice patterns. Cofeeding opportunities were then alternated with individual choice tests. In Experiment 2,2 other pairs were added. Each animal was again offered simultaneous choice tests between marshmallows and almonds. Food aversion conditioning was used to create different choice patterns for mothers and offspring. After cofeeding and choice tests, the differences in choice patterns disappeared in both experiments. The changes after contact with the other's eating pattern during cofeeding was as follows: foods consumed by either came to be eaten by both; foods consumed by both continued to be eaten by both; and foods consumed by neither continued to be ignored. The results provide evidence for social transmission of food preferences in this species. 相似文献