首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   43篇
  免费   0篇
  43篇
  2018年   1篇
  2016年   2篇
  2015年   1篇
  2014年   1篇
  2013年   5篇
  2012年   1篇
  2010年   2篇
  2009年   1篇
  2008年   7篇
  2007年   2篇
  2004年   2篇
  2002年   2篇
  2001年   2篇
  2000年   1篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   1篇
  1993年   2篇
  1991年   1篇
  1990年   1篇
  1989年   1篇
  1986年   1篇
  1984年   1篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   1篇
  1972年   1篇
排序方式: 共有43条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
A detection theoretic analysis was employed to examine sensitivity and response bias in two modalities. In Experiment 1, 6 tasters made same-different judgments about the concentration of either sucrose or quinine in pairs of tonic water samples. The beverages were colored, but color was not predictive of the concentration of the sweet or bitter ingredient. When same-different ratings were collapsed to approximate the outcome of a categorical decision, tasters with poorer sensitivity appear to have adopted more extreme response criteria than did tasters with greater sensitivity, irrespective of taste quality, color, or whether pairs of solutions comprised the same or different colors. In Experiment 2, 3 individuals discriminated pairs of 1000-Hz sinusoids differing in amplitude. Six amplitude differences were tested. Rating-scale versions of two paradigms: The single-interval yes-no task and the two-interval same-different task were used to measure sensitivity and bias. There was a preponderance of "same" responses in the same-different task. Estimates of bias obtained from collapsed ratings in both tasks were unaffected by sensitivity, but a consideration of the range over which sets of criteria were spread suggested a general tendency toward more conservative response biases as sensitivity declined.  相似文献   
32.
Tasters selected the odd stimulus from among sets of three samples of party dip. Two samples came from one batch, and one sample came from another batch. The physicochemical difference between the batches consisted of the presence or absence of added salt. Two different tests of discriminability were undertaken by the same subjects with the same stimuli: the triangle test and the three-alternative forced-choice (3-AFC) method. Although different numbers of correct selections were obtained in the two tasks, an index of discriminability,d’, had the same value when the data were analyzed in accordance with the Thurstone-Ura and signal-detection models, respectively. The average data support Frijters’s (1979b) contention that different models of the discrimination process are appropriate to the results of the triangular and the 3-AFC procedures. Further analysis of the data revealed that discrimination was poorer for trios containing one physicochemically weak stimulus and two stronger stimuli than it was for trios containing one stronger stimulus and two weak stimuli. A two-signal 3-AFC task was undertaken by some subjects, andd’ estimates from this task were lower than expected on the basis of performance in the other tasks.  相似文献   
33.
The 6 experiments reported here tested the effects of various category relations on automatic semantic priming in 20 Alzheimer's disease (AD), 20 older control, and 22 younger control subjects. The tasks were either word pronunciation or lexical decision; the prime-target stimulus-onset asynchrony (SOA) was always 250 ms. A variety of category relationships between prime and target were examined: highly associated category comembers, subordinate-superordinate or superordinate-subordinate pairs, and pairs selected on the basis of category typically to form typical-typical, atypical-typical, typical-atypical, and atypical-atypical pairings. Both for AD versus older control subjects and for older versus younger control subjects, no significant group differences were found in the magnitude of overall semantic priming or in the effects on priming of factors pertaining to the prime-target relationship.  相似文献   
34.
35.
Emotional experiences can bring about personal growth. For instance, feeling guilty may prompt one to learn from a mistake, and this learning can bring about different and better future behavior. Three studies found that belief in free will facilitated learning from emotional experiences, as inducing participants to disbelieve in free will was associated with reduced learning. Emotional responsiveness, as defined by low psychopathy scores, also facilitated learning from emotional experiences (Studies 2 and 3). The degree of learning associated with emotional experiences was measured by self-rating (Study 1), independent evaluations of lessons learned (Study 2), and whether participants joined a campus recycling program (after being made to feel guilty about an environmental transgression; Study 3).  相似文献   
36.
The present research tested two competing hypotheses: (1) as money cues activate an exchange orientation to social relations, money cues harm prosocial responses in communal and collectivistic settings; (2) as money can be used to help close others, money cues increase helping in communal or collectivistic settings. In a culture, characterized by strong helping norms, money cues reduced the quality of help given (Experiment 1), and lowered perceived moral obligation to help (Experiment 2). In communal relationships, money reminders decreased willingness to help romantic partners (Experiment 3). This effect was attenuated among people high on communal strength, although money cues made them upset with help requests (Experiment 4). Thus, the harmful effects of money on prosocial responses appear robust.  相似文献   
37.
Thresholds for a masked 1-kHz tone were obtained in single sessions from 60 inexperienced listeners. A two-alternative forced-choice procedure was used in conjunction with one of three adaptive psychophysical techniques. These techniques comprised two staircase techniques targeting either 70.7% or 79.4% correct detection (Staircase-71 and Staircase-79), and parameter estimation by sequential testing (PEST), targeting 80% correct detection. Listeners were provided with a rationale for maintaining concentration at weak signal levels. Similar threshold values were obtained from the Staircase-79 and PEST groups in equal numbers of trials. The degree of oscillation in the level of the signal around the value finally chosen as the threshold was comparable for both staircase techniques. Subsequent fixed-level testing did not provide a true indication of the subjects' capabilities. The amount by which percent correct in fixed-level testing differed from expectations based on adaptive testing varied among the techniques. Additional thresholds were obtained in a second session from 30 of the original subjects. Thresholds with both staircase techniques improved by about 1 dB on retest, while thresholds with PEST were constant across sessions. The variability of the data compared well with that from studies involving experienced listeners.  相似文献   
38.
Reliable taste detection thresholds in humans are difficult to obtain for either research or clinical purposes because ordinary taste stimuli cannot be generated and presented by means of a computer. However, in investigations of taste dysfunction, electrical taste stimulation (electrogustometry) is sometimes employed. Minute anodal direct currents are applied manually to the tongue, giving rise to an acid taste experience through the liberation of protons into the saliva. This experience accords with recent findings on the mechanisms underlying sour taste perception. The device described here is a safe and accurate constant-current device, operating under computer control at very low amperages, making it possible to apply modern psychophysical procedures to the measurement of evoked taste thresholds.  相似文献   
39.
A system has been developed to sense and record eye point-of-gaze for on-line computer processing, using a Mackworth camera in conjunction with a closed-circuit television system. System optics and electronics are described, together with computer software for system calibration and automatic artifact removal.  相似文献   
40.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号