全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1946篇 |
免费 | 196篇 |
国内免费 | 70篇 |
专业分类
2212篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 20篇 |
2022年 | 40篇 |
2021年 | 40篇 |
2020年 | 55篇 |
2019年 | 38篇 |
2018年 | 43篇 |
2017年 | 52篇 |
2016年 | 75篇 |
2015年 | 45篇 |
2014年 | 70篇 |
2013年 | 163篇 |
2012年 | 81篇 |
2011年 | 98篇 |
2010年 | 78篇 |
2009年 | 70篇 |
2008年 | 77篇 |
2007年 | 67篇 |
2006年 | 80篇 |
2005年 | 56篇 |
2004年 | 58篇 |
2003年 | 45篇 |
2002年 | 50篇 |
2001年 | 48篇 |
2000年 | 42篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 22篇 |
1997年 | 18篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 25篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 30篇 |
1990年 | 24篇 |
1989年 | 20篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1986年 | 24篇 |
1985年 | 29篇 |
1984年 | 21篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1979年 | 26篇 |
1978年 | 19篇 |
1977年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 24篇 |
1973年 | 22篇 |
1972年 | 19篇 |
1971年 | 21篇 |
1969年 | 18篇 |
排序方式: 共有2212条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
182.
Bertram D. Cohen 《Group》2000,24(1):93-103
A futuristic essay should be written by two persons so as to invoke Bion's basic assumption pairing (Bion, 1964) with its connotations of hope, no matter how irrational. Accordingly, I split my self in two parts, one, the hard-headed realist, and the other, the prophetic visionary. As idealized identities, neither comes easily to me. But, perhaps heeding the two has permitted the articulation of some presentable notions about group psychotherapy and its evolving fate. 相似文献
183.
Diedrichsen J Ivry RB Cohen A Danziger S 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2000,26(1):113-126
Four experiments were conducted using a flanker task with 1 distractor appearing either on the left or right side of a central target. Responses were made on a keyboard aligned parallel to the displays. A larger flanker effect was obtained when the distractor was on the same side as the response. Two factors account for this asymmetry. First, when the flanker and target are identical, the 2 form a group that is assigned a spatial tag, creating a form of the Simon effect on the basis of the compatibility between the response keys and the group. Second, preparation of a lateralized response appears to entail a shift of visual attention in the corresponding direction, thus enhancing processing of the flanker on the response side. Consistent with the 2nd hypothesis, participants were more likely to correctly recognize letters that were briefly presented at the distractor position on the same side as the response. 相似文献
184.
185.
Selma A. Lewis Jim Johnson Patricia Cohen Marc Garcia Carmen Noemi Velez 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1988,16(4):459-471
Twenty-six suicide attempters, 9 to 18 years of age, were compared with 725 nonattempters on measures of school achievement, educational goals, socioeconomic status, and depression. As has been found in studies using clinical samples, suicide attempters in our communitybased sample had significantly lower school achievement than nonattempters. The relationship between attempted suicide and low school achievement seemed to be explained by the effects of depression. Lower-SES youth tended to be more likely to attempt suicide than higher-SES youths. However, SES appeared to have little effect on the relationship between attempted suicide and school achievement. Results also suggest that children of mothers with low educational goals for them may be more at risk for attempted suicide than youths whose mothers have higher educational goals for their children.This research was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health, Grant No. MH36971. 相似文献
186.
Jerry L. Cohen 《Motivation and emotion》1979,3(1):19-33
Video-recording procedures have been used in previous research as a motivational manipulation. The present study investigated the permanent recording of behavior as a manipulation of evaluation apprehension and its effects upon behavioral performance and upon autonomic arousal. Twenty-four subjects (12 males, 12 females) were randomly assigned to one of four conditions formed by completely crossing two levels of audience status (peer or authority) with two levels of behavioral record (non-permanent or permanent). Another 24 subjects were assigned to an external control group to the 2×2 design. Subjects' performance on a hidden-word task and their arousal level (as assessed by the Palmar Sweat Index) were measured. Analyses of variance results showed a significant performance (p<.05) and a significant arousal (p<.05) difference between the behavioral record conditions as predicted. The control group was also found to be significantly (p<.05) different from the experimental groups on the behavioral performance measure. The results were related to previous research findings and interpreted within the context of the evaluation apprehension notion presented within social facilitation research.The author would like to thank Angelia Mellors, Valerie K. Golish, and Judy Cohen for serving as experiments, and Susan L. Doman and Cheryl Primmer for scoring the PSI measures. 相似文献
187.
Sixty-nine midrange responders on the Introversion-Extroversion (IE) scale of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ) were given an experimental measure to assess tendency toward ambivalence (i.e., both strongly introverted and strongly extraverted responses) vs. moderation (i.e., midrange responses), and degree of importance given to IE types of activities. The participants were divided into those showing ambivalence vs. those showing moderate responses, and into those reporting high vs. low importance of such activities. As was predicted, the ambivalent group showed significantly higher scores on the neuroticism scale of the EPQ, while the low importance group tended to show more psychoticism, but not to a significant degree. Both of the midrange scores were unrelated to the Bem Sex Role Inventory. Based on the findings, at least two groups are posited in the midrange of the IE dimension: an ambivalent group with mixed strong introversive and extraversive tendencies, and an ambiverted group with midrange scores. 相似文献
188.
Leslie B. Cohen Leslie J. Rundell Barbara A. Spellman & Cara H. Cashon 《Psychological science》1999,10(5):412-418
Three experiments examined infants' and adults' perception of causal sequences of events. In a causal-chain sequence, the first action causes a second action that then causes a final outcome; in a temporal-chain sequence, the first two actions are independent and the second action causes a final outcome. Infants and adults were shown the same event sequences; infants were tested using a visual habituation paradigm, whereas adults were given a questionnaire. Experiment 1 indicated that 15-month-old infants perceive the primary cause of the final outcome to be the first action in a causal chain but the second action in a temporal chain. Experiment 2 showed that adults interpret the causal sequences in a manner similar to that of 15-month-olds. Finally, Experiment 3 showed that 10-month-old infants do not yet perceive causal sequences in the same manner as 15-month-olds and adults. These results are interpreted in terms of both infants' developing knowledge of causal events and adults' attributions of causality in complex events. 相似文献
189.
Age differences in memory for the source of memories were investigated using two different experimental paradigms. Experiment 1 used a reality monitoring paradigm. A series of actions were either performed, imagined, or watched, and subjects were later tested for their ability to recognize the actions and identify their origins. Elderly subjects made more false positive responses than did young subjects, and they made more source confusion errors, attributing actions to the wrong sources. Both new and imagined actions were most often misclassified as watched. Experiment 2 used an eyewitness testimony paradigm. After watching a film, subjects read a written version of the story. A recognition test showed that elderly subjects were more often misled by false information in the story than were the younger subjects, and were more confident that their erroneous responses were correct. The findings suggest that a decline in memory for sources may diminish the accuracy of elderly witnesses. 相似文献