首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   20517篇
  免费   510篇
  国内免费   192篇
  2012年   1978篇
  2011年   2231篇
  2010年   523篇
  2009年   240篇
  2008年   1777篇
  2007年   1781篇
  2006年   1615篇
  2005年   1370篇
  2004年   1165篇
  2003年   1130篇
  2002年   1017篇
  2001年   720篇
  2000年   932篇
  1999年   373篇
  1998年   109篇
  1997年   91篇
  1996年   101篇
  1995年   56篇
  1994年   91篇
  1993年   143篇
  1992年   116篇
  1991年   118篇
  1990年   111篇
  1989年   109篇
  1988年   128篇
  1987年   142篇
  1986年   108篇
  1985年   136篇
  1984年   135篇
  1983年   152篇
  1982年   105篇
  1981年   107篇
  1980年   91篇
  1979年   101篇
  1978年   91篇
  1966年   49篇
  1965年   137篇
  1964年   106篇
  1963年   127篇
  1962年   56篇
  1959年   57篇
  1958年   207篇
  1957年   167篇
  1956年   143篇
  1955年   104篇
  1954年   91篇
  1953年   101篇
  1952年   72篇
  1951年   64篇
  1950年   60篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
论概念     
1.知识分为(a)普遍承认的概念(太阳、月亮、智慧、灵魂)和(b)有待证明的概念(例如确证宇宙是由包含在另一球体中的球面组成的;或者是在时间中被创造的)。每个概念都必定有一个更普遍的概念在它之前存在着。但是,那个概念的最终建立却无需和在它之先的概念相关联。必然的存在和可能的存在就是这样的概念。这样的概念无需人们去构想一个包含着它们的东西,它们本身就是清楚、明白的自明概  相似文献   
142.
许多人主张,R.罗蒂是在传统哲学的范围中进行论证的,尽管他的论点恰恰相反。本文的目的,就是针对这些人的看法提出一个建议:应把罗蒂看作是处于哲学传统之外的重要思想家,一个处于哲学传统之外的批判者。我的建议所围绕的中心,是略述罗蒂的实用主义如何不同于人们较为熟知的那些实用主义。一般承认,实用主义是一种重要的哲学观点,一种经常被援引的观点。但难得有人对这一观点作出清楚的说明。这是由于,当人们援引实用主义时,往往把它与包含有真理符合论的主张和观点加以对比,因而只是对它作了颇为有限的论述。最近罗蒂以几种方式援引了实用主义。通过这些方式,他驳斥了真理符合论,甚至一般地驳斥了需要有任何真理理论,他诉诸实用主义,运用成效  相似文献   
143.
在阶级社会里,宗教和哲学唯心主义之间的相似和密切联系,是毋庸置疑的。无论是宗教还是哲学唯心主义,都是对世界的一种不正确的看法,都是一种错误的、歪曲的、颠倒的世界观。宗教和哲学唯心主义都反映这些和那些阶级的利益,它们过去和现在多半是阶级社会里的思想斗争中的同盟军。以上这些观点都是下述看法的依据,即在我们的文献里,充分而又广泛地提到宗教和哲学唯心主义是一些不同的现象,但就其本质来说,又是同一  相似文献   
144.
一、导言我认为历史唯物主义就是马克思在其著名的《<政治经济学批判>序言》(以下简称《序言》——译者)中所阐述的、在《资本论》和马克思以及马克思主义者其他许多著作中举出作为例证的那种观点。我将用几句导言开始我的论述,然后概略地介绍一下这个理论,最后我要力辩,尽管这个观点确实具有许多吸引人的地方,但它过分限制了对历史进行合理探讨的范围,以致最后成为不可接受的了。  相似文献   
145.
The timing of natural prehension movements   总被引:39,自引:0,他引:39  
Prehension movements were studied by film in 7 adult subjects. Transportation of the hand to the target-object location had features very similar to any aiming arm movement, that is, it involved a fast-velocity initial phase and a low-velocity final phase. The peak velocity of the movement was highly correlated with its amplitude, although total movement duration tended to remain invariant when target distance was changed. The low-velocity phase consistently began after about 75% of movement time had elapsed. This ration was maintained for different movement amplitudes. Formation of the finger grip occurred during hand transportation. Fingers were first stretched and then began to close in anticipation to contact with the object. The onset of the closure phase was highly correlated to the beginning of the low velocity phase of transportation. This pattern for both transportation and finger grip formation was maintained in conditions whether visual feedback from the moving limb was present or not. Implications of these findings for the central programming of multisegmental movements are discussed.  相似文献   
146.
Broome J 《Ethics》1984,95(1):38-55
This article considers what justification can be found for selecting randomly and in what circumstances it applies, including that of selecting patients to be treated by a scarce medical procedure. The author demonstrates that balancing the merits of fairness, common good, equal rights, and equal chance as they apply in various situations frequently leads to the conclusion that random selection may not be the most appropriate mode of selection. Broome acknowledges that, in the end, we may be forced to conclude that the only merit of random selection is the political one of guarding against partiality and oppression.  相似文献   
147.
148.
幼儿不同情绪状态对其智力操作的影响   总被引:3,自引:1,他引:2  
本实验结果显示:1)在16至18个月的婴儿中,愉快和痛苦情绪对其问题解决的操作有不同的影响。愉快组被试比痛苦组被试的操作效果明显优越;体现在“直接抓取”、“注视”和“顺利获得”三种操作策略中。2)愉快和痛苦两种情绪的不同强度与操作效果之间的相关是不同的。愉快组显示为一种曲线相关,即过高或过低的情绪唤醒不如适中的唤醒水平使操作效果达到最优。痛苦组显示为一种直线相关,即痛苦强度越大,操作效果越差。  相似文献   
149.
This study examined the relationship between job stress and employees' performance and withdrawal behavior among nurses (N = 440) in two hospitals in a metropolitan Canadian city on the east coast. Job stressors assessed included role ambiguity, role overload, role conflict, and resource inadequacy. Employees' performance was operationalized in terms of job performance, motivation, and patient care skill. Withdrawal behaviors assessed were absenteeism, tardiness, and anticipated turnover. Multiple regressions, curvilinear correlation coefficients, and canonical correlations were computed to test the nature of the relationship between stressors and the criterion variables of the study. In general, data were more supportive of the negative linear relationship between stress and performance than for positive linear or curvilinear relationship. However, the stressor role ambiguity did exhibit a monotonic nonlinear relationship with a number of criterion variables. Employees' professional and organizational commitment were proposed to moderate the stress-performance relationship. However, the data only partially supported the role of the moderators.  相似文献   
150.
Each of three pigeons was studied first under a standard fixed-interval schedule. With the fixed interval held constant, the schedule was changed to a second-order schedule in which the response unit was the behavior on a small fixed-ratio schedule (first a fixed-ratio 10 and then a fixed-ratio 20 schedule). That is, every completion of the fixed-ratio schedule produced a 0.7-sec darkening of the key and reset the response count to zero for the next ratio. The first fixed-ratio completed after the fixed-interval schedule elapsed produced the 0.7-sec blackout followed immediately by food. These manipulations were carried out under two different fixed-interval durations for each bird ranging from 3 min to 12 min. The standard fixed-interval schedules produced the typical pause after reinforcement followed by responding at a moderate rate until the next reinforcement. The second-order schedules also engendered a pause after reinforcement, but responding occurred in bursts separated by brief pauses after each blackout. For a particular fixed-interval duration, post-reinforcement pauses increased slightly as the number of pecks in the response unit increased despite large differences in the rate and pattern of key pecking. Post-reinforcement pause increased with the fixed-interval duration under all response units. These data confirm that the allocation of time between pausing and responding is relatively independent of the rate and topography of responding after the pause.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号