首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   75936篇
  免费   3048篇
  国内免费   30篇
  79014篇
  2020年   822篇
  2019年   964篇
  2018年   1399篇
  2017年   1411篇
  2016年   1493篇
  2015年   1061篇
  2014年   1312篇
  2013年   6269篇
  2012年   2393篇
  2011年   2438篇
  2010年   1504篇
  2009年   1511篇
  2008年   2170篇
  2007年   2086篇
  2006年   1936篇
  2005年   1628篇
  2004年   1660篇
  2003年   1605篇
  2002年   1571篇
  2001年   2387篇
  2000年   2245篇
  1999年   1718篇
  1998年   838篇
  1997年   763篇
  1996年   810篇
  1995年   743篇
  1992年   1467篇
  1991年   1360篇
  1990年   1331篇
  1989年   1274篇
  1988年   1260篇
  1987年   1183篇
  1986年   1209篇
  1985年   1240篇
  1984年   1035篇
  1983年   897篇
  1979年   1092篇
  1978年   844篇
  1977年   702篇
  1976年   711篇
  1975年   880篇
  1974年   1036篇
  1973年   1092篇
  1972年   898篇
  1971年   859篇
  1970年   777篇
  1969年   790篇
  1968年   975篇
  1967年   881篇
  1966年   867篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
951.
952.

Galef and his colleagues have repeatedly shown that one rat may transfer information regarding the type of food it has consumed to other conspecifics. Such experiments typically have been conducted in wire-mesh cages or a wooden maze. The present experiments sought to extend this paradigm to the open-field foraging situation having six food patches to choose from. Following interaction with a demonstrator that had consumed either a cocoa or a cinnamon diet, single observers (Experiment 1) were tested in the foraging situation. Food-consumption scores indicated that observers consumed significantly more of their specific demonstrator’s diet than a second diet that was available also. Experiment 2 involved the simultaneous testing of two observers in the foraging laboratory. In Experiment 3 two observers were once again tested, but each had been provided a different food-type message prior to foraging. Positive results, mirroring those of Experiment 1, were obtained in both Experiments 2 and 3. The results of these three experiments underscore the robustness of this phenomenon and its generalizability to other testing conditions.

  相似文献   
953.
Book Notes     
The Psychological Record -  相似文献   
954.
Intellectual functioning and aggression   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
In a 22-year study, data were collected on aggressiveness and intellectual functioning in more than 600 subjects, their parents, and their children. Both aggression and intellectual functioning are reasonably stable in a subject's lifetime and perpetuate themselves across generations and within marriage pairs. Aggression in childhood was shown to interfere with the development of intellectual functioning and to be predictive of poorer intellectual achievement as an adult. Early IQ was related to early subject aggression but did not predict changes in aggression after age 8. On the other hand, differences between early IQ and intellectual achievement in middle adulthood were predictable from early aggressive behavior. A dual-process model was offered to explain the relation between intellectual functioning and aggressive behavior. We hypothesized that low intelligence makes the learning of aggressive responses more likely at an early age, and this aggressive behavior makes continued intellectual development more difficult.  相似文献   
955.
956.
A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction   总被引:64,自引:0,他引:64  
  相似文献   
957.
A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
  相似文献   
958.
959.
Cognitive maps as orienting schemata   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
A "point-to-unseen-targets" task was used to test two theories about the nature of cognitive mapping. The hypothesis that a cognitive map is like a "picture in the head" predicts that (a) the cognitive map should have a preferred orientation and (b) all coded locations should be equally available. These predictions were confirmed in Experiments 1 and 3 when targets were cities in the northeastern United States and learning was from a map. The theory that a cognitive map is an orienting schema predicts that the cognitive map should have no preferred orientation and that targets in front of the body should be localized faster than targets behind the body. These predictions were confirmed in Experiments 1 and 2 when targets were local landmarks that had been learned via direct experience. In Experiment 3, when cities in the Northeast were targets and geographical knowledge had been acquired, in part, by traveling in the Northeast, the observed latency profiles were not as predicted by either theory of cognitive mapping. The results suggest that orienting schemata direct orientation with respect to local environments, but that orientation with respect to large geographical regions is supported by a different type of cognitive structure.  相似文献   
960.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号