全文获取类型
收费全文 | 86830篇 |
免费 | 3608篇 |
国内免费 | 33篇 |
专业分类
90471篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 969篇 |
2019年 | 1131篇 |
2018年 | 1582篇 |
2017年 | 1617篇 |
2016年 | 1711篇 |
2015年 | 1255篇 |
2014年 | 1481篇 |
2013年 | 6971篇 |
2012年 | 2807篇 |
2011年 | 2792篇 |
2010年 | 1715篇 |
2009年 | 1694篇 |
2008年 | 2431篇 |
2007年 | 2389篇 |
2006年 | 2140篇 |
2005年 | 1877篇 |
2004年 | 1877篇 |
2003年 | 1766篇 |
2002年 | 1736篇 |
2001年 | 2784篇 |
2000年 | 2628篇 |
1999年 | 1984篇 |
1998年 | 936篇 |
1997年 | 814篇 |
1996年 | 874篇 |
1994年 | 804篇 |
1992年 | 1738篇 |
1991年 | 1621篇 |
1990年 | 1627篇 |
1989年 | 1506篇 |
1988年 | 1476篇 |
1987年 | 1363篇 |
1986年 | 1413篇 |
1985年 | 1530篇 |
1984年 | 1190篇 |
1983年 | 1086篇 |
1979年 | 1298篇 |
1978年 | 899篇 |
1977年 | 803篇 |
1976年 | 813篇 |
1975年 | 1072篇 |
1974年 | 1235篇 |
1973年 | 1283篇 |
1972年 | 1115篇 |
1971年 | 1026篇 |
1970年 | 959篇 |
1969年 | 951篇 |
1968年 | 1237篇 |
1967年 | 1103篇 |
1966年 | 1006篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
172.
173.
174.
D M Buss 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,54(4):616-628
Darwin's theory of sexual selection suggests that individuals compete with members of their own sex for reproductively relevant resources held by members of the opposite sex. Four empirical studies were conducted to identify tactics of intrasexual mate competition and to test four evolution-based hypotheses. A preliminary study yielded a taxonomy of tactics. Study 1 used close-friend observers to report performance frequencies of 23 tactics to test the hypotheses. Study 2 replicated Study 1's results by using a different data source and subject population. Study 3 provided an independent test of the hypotheses in assessing the perceived effectiveness of each tactic for male and female actors. Although the basic hypotheses were supported across all three studies, there were several predictive failures and unanticipated findings. Discussion centers on the heuristic as well as predictive role of evolutionary theory, and on implications for other arenas of intrasexual competition. 相似文献
175.
Subjects (N= 28) performing a complex disjunctive concept-formation task on a microcomputer were given the opportunity to seek help following failure to correctly identify concepts. The proportion of those seeking help was significantly greater when the source of help was the computer itself (86%) rather than another person (36%). Consistent with past research, those who did not ask for help gave reasons other than the potential negative social consequences of doing so. Self-report data and a systematic test for the presence of experimenter-induced demand ruled out several possible sources of confounding. The results have implications for designers of time-sharing and networked computer systems. 相似文献
176.
Neuroticism, extraversion, and related traits in adult twins reared apart and reared together 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
N L Pedersen R Plomin G E McClearn L Friberg 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,55(6):950-957
The relative importance of genetic and environmental factors for neuroticism, extraversion, impulsivity, and monotony avoidance were estimated in a sample of 99 monozygotic and 229 dizygotic pairs of twins reared apart (TRA) and a matched sample of 160 monozygotic and 212 dizygotic pairs of twins reared together (TRT). The average age was 58.6 (SD = 13.6); 72% of the twins were 50 or older. Model-fitting analyses verified the importance of genetic factors for all four measures; from 23% to 45% of the total variation was attributable to genetic sources. There was considerable evidence that these factors were operating in a nonadditive manner for extraversion and impulsivity. Shared environment accounted for less than 10% of the variance; some evidence for selective placement was found for neuroticism. 相似文献
177.
H P de Groot M I Gwynn N P Spanos 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,54(6):1049-1053
Council, Kirsch, and Hafner (1986) obtained empirical support for the hypothesis that significant correlations between questionnaire measures of absorption and hypnotic susceptibility are an artifact of subjects' beliefs about their own hypnotizability. We tested this hypothesis in a two-session experiment. During Session 1, subjects completed questionnaire measures of absorption, mystical experience, daydreaming frequency, and paranormal beliefs. During Session 2, subjects were tested for hypnotic susceptibility. Subjects were also exposed to one of three information manipulations: They were told about hypnotic testing either before or after filling out the questionnaires or were not told about hypnotic testing. The information manipulation moderated the prediction of susceptibility by the questionnaire measures for women, but not for men. For women, scores on the absorption questionnaire predicted susceptibility only when subjects were informed about hypnotic testing. In the told-after condition, this effect generalized to all of the remaining questionnaire measures. For men, none of the questionnaires was a reliable predictor of susceptibility. 相似文献
178.
Traits and states are concepts that people use to both describe and understand themselves and others. We show that people view these concepts as prototype-based categories that have a graded internal structure and fuzzy boundaries and identify a set of attributes that define the prototypical cores of two categories: traits and states. Prototypical traits are stable, long-lasting, and internally caused. Prototypical states are temporary, brief, and caused by external circumstances. These prototypes are not defined by averages, as the family-resemblance principle would suggest, but by ideal (or extreme) attribute values. Like other ideal-based categories, traits and states serve particular goals. Trait concepts permit people to predict the present from the past; state concepts identify those behaviors that can be controlled by manipulating the situation. These two complementary schemas are part of the extensive theory of psychological causality that is implicit in language. Abstract social concepts differ from object categories in their category standards, the nature of their attributes, and the interrelations among those attributes. 相似文献
179.
The recognition of threatening facial stimuli 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
J Aronoff A M Barclay L A Stevenson 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,54(4):647-655
Two studies examined the information that defines a threatening facial display. The first study identified those facial characteristics that distinguish between representations of threatening and nonthreatening facial displays. Masks that presented either threatening or nonthreatening facial displays were obtained from a number of non-Western cultures and scored for the presence of those facial features that discriminated between such displays in the drawings of two American samples. Threatening masks contained a significantly higher number of these characteristics across all cultures examined. The second study determined whether the information provided by the facial display might be more primary nonrepresentational visual patterns than facial features with obvious denotative meaning (e.g., diagonal lines rather than downturned eyebrows). The subjective response to sets of diagonal, angular, and curvilinear visual stimuli revealed that the nonrepresentational features of angularity and diagonality in the visual stimulus appeared to have the ability to evoke the subjective responses that convey the meaning of threat. 相似文献
180.
Personality in adulthood: a six-year longitudinal study of self-reports and spouse ratings on the NEO Personality Inventory 总被引:20,自引:0,他引:20
Previous longitudinal studies of personality in adulthood have been limited in the range of traits examined, have chiefly made use of self-reports, and have frequently included only men. In this study, self-reports (N = 983) and spouse ratings (N = 167) were gathered on the NEO Personality Inventory (Costa & McCrae, 1985b), which measures all five of the major dimensions of normal personality. Cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses on data from men and women aged 21 to 96 years showed evidence of small declines in Activity, Positive Emotions, and openness to Actions that might be attributed to maturation, but none of these effects was replicated in sequential analyses. The 20 other scales examined showed no consistent pattern of maturational effects. In contrast, retest stability was quite high for all five dimensions in self-reports and for the three dimensions measured at both times in spouse ratings. Comparable levels of stability were seen for men and women and for younger and older subjects. The data support the position that personality is stable after age 30. 相似文献