首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   64754篇
  免费   2743篇
  国内免费   25篇
  2020年   782篇
  2019年   910篇
  2018年   1327篇
  2017年   1341篇
  2016年   1388篇
  2015年   1006篇
  2014年   1195篇
  2013年   5489篇
  2012年   2199篇
  2011年   2234篇
  2010年   1353篇
  2009年   1337篇
  2008年   1907篇
  2007年   1857篇
  2006年   1725篇
  2005年   1405篇
  2004年   1447篇
  2003年   1380篇
  2002年   1314篇
  2001年   2199篇
  2000年   2079篇
  1999年   1529篇
  1998年   707篇
  1997年   596篇
  1996年   690篇
  1995年   619篇
  1994年   606篇
  1993年   598篇
  1992年   1260篇
  1991年   1183篇
  1990年   1146篇
  1989年   1109篇
  1988年   1095篇
  1987年   1021篇
  1986年   994篇
  1985年   1044篇
  1984年   860篇
  1983年   715篇
  1979年   888篇
  1978年   640篇
  1975年   718篇
  1974年   818篇
  1973年   859篇
  1972年   733篇
  1971年   685篇
  1970年   588篇
  1969年   599篇
  1968年   769篇
  1967年   648篇
  1966年   625篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Optimal timing and the Weber function   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
992.
A psychomotor stimulant theory of addiction   总被引:64,自引:0,他引:64  
  相似文献   
993.
A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
  相似文献   
994.
995.
Cognitive maps as orienting schemata   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
A "point-to-unseen-targets" task was used to test two theories about the nature of cognitive mapping. The hypothesis that a cognitive map is like a "picture in the head" predicts that (a) the cognitive map should have a preferred orientation and (b) all coded locations should be equally available. These predictions were confirmed in Experiments 1 and 3 when targets were cities in the northeastern United States and learning was from a map. The theory that a cognitive map is an orienting schema predicts that the cognitive map should have no preferred orientation and that targets in front of the body should be localized faster than targets behind the body. These predictions were confirmed in Experiments 1 and 2 when targets were local landmarks that had been learned via direct experience. In Experiment 3, when cities in the Northeast were targets and geographical knowledge had been acquired, in part, by traveling in the Northeast, the observed latency profiles were not as predicted by either theory of cognitive mapping. The results suggest that orienting schemata direct orientation with respect to local environments, but that orientation with respect to large geographical regions is supported by a different type of cognitive structure.  相似文献   
996.
Two visuospatial tasks, the WAIS Block Design and the Street Gestalt Completion Test, were administered to men and women with and without unilateral cerebral lesions. These two tasks represent different categories of visuospatial functions. The Street test is a visual-perceptual gestalt task, requiring the closure of fragmented pictures, whereas Block Design is an analytical, manipulospatial task requiring rotation of spatial coordinates. For the non-brain-damaged group, the men showed a nonsignificant trend toward better Block Design performance relative to the women, whereas there was no sex-related difference in Street performance. For the brain-damaged groups, patients with right hemisphere lesions performed significantly worse than patients with left hemisphere lesions on both the Street test and Block Design, indicating that both tasks were more sensitive to right hemisphere functioning. There was, however, a significant sex X side of lesion interaction on Block Design only, with the men showing a more asymmetrical pattern of scores. These results suggest that sex differences in functional lateralization may underlie sex differences in visuospatial ability, and that sex differences in functional lateralization may be present for only certain visuospatial processes.  相似文献   
997.
Stimulus categorization by brain-damaged patients   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Patients with localized insult to the right hemisphere, or to either the anterior or the posterior portion of the left hemisphere, as well as neurologically intact controls, evaluated stimuli on a seven-point rating scale for their degree of category membership. The stimuli were taken from one of two continua, one composed of fruit and vegetable items, and the other of items differing in hue and shape. Different subsets of stimuli provided different contexts for the judgments of category membership. The two left-hemisphere groups showed anomalies in categorizing the fruit and vegetable items but not the perceptual items, while the reverse was true for the right-hemisphere patients. Moreover, both left-hemisphere groups demonstrated context effects in their judgments of the representativeness of the fruit and vegetable items, but differed in the way in which they responded to changing contexts. Left posterior patients demonstrated weak category boundaries and even reclassified items. In contrast, patients with left anterior damage showed highly categorical responses and less differentiation of items within a category. All groups showed striking context effects in judgments of perceptual items in terms of changes in representativeness ratings and the location of a category boundary. Alternative interpretations of the results are offered.  相似文献   
998.
Teacher-rated adjustment differences among young elementary school children from (a) a recent sample and a 1974 cohort, and (b) urban/suburban, male/female, and grade-level subgroups were examined. A problem behavior checklist (Classroom Adjustment Rating Scale) and a school competence measure (Health Resources Inventory) for 974 children from 5 urban and 5 suburban schools were completed by 101 first- to fourth-grade teachers. Children from the more current cohort were rated as significantly more maladjusted than those from the earlier sample on 8 of 10 adjustment variables. In the recent sample, girls and suburban children evidenced a greater number of school competencies and fewer problem behaviors than boys and urban children, respectively. The implications of these findings for the future use of the teacher rating scales are discussed.  相似文献   
999.
The urban biases of empirical research on gay men, women, and families have resulted in minimal knowledge about gay people in rural settings. The diversity of lives of rural gay women and men and the variety of patterns of meeting the challenges of rural living are described. Processes of help-seeking and help-giving are discussed and the need for a helping community of family, friends, and caring others is affirmed. Collaboration between rural gay people and rural community psychologists is suggested to promote the development of helping communities for gay people and thereby initiate a process of change in rural settings.  相似文献   
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号