全文获取类型
收费全文 | 75849篇 |
免费 | 1345篇 |
国内免费 | 5篇 |
专业分类
77199篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 856篇 |
2019年 | 980篇 |
2018年 | 1373篇 |
2017年 | 1439篇 |
2016年 | 1506篇 |
2015年 | 1058篇 |
2014年 | 1292篇 |
2013年 | 6182篇 |
2012年 | 2402篇 |
2011年 | 2462篇 |
2010年 | 1504篇 |
2009年 | 1501篇 |
2008年 | 2111篇 |
2007年 | 2051篇 |
2006年 | 1898篇 |
2005年 | 1596篇 |
2004年 | 1640篇 |
2003年 | 1549篇 |
2002年 | 1526篇 |
2001年 | 2436篇 |
2000年 | 2298篇 |
1999年 | 1758篇 |
1998年 | 837篇 |
1997年 | 707篇 |
1996年 | 764篇 |
1994年 | 702篇 |
1993年 | 695篇 |
1992年 | 1428篇 |
1991年 | 1319篇 |
1990年 | 1335篇 |
1989年 | 1258篇 |
1988年 | 1228篇 |
1987年 | 1172篇 |
1986年 | 1175篇 |
1985年 | 1246篇 |
1984年 | 990篇 |
1983年 | 874篇 |
1982年 | 685篇 |
1979年 | 1068篇 |
1978年 | 809篇 |
1975年 | 902篇 |
1974年 | 998篇 |
1973年 | 1009篇 |
1972年 | 864篇 |
1971年 | 812篇 |
1970年 | 709篇 |
1969年 | 755篇 |
1968年 | 906篇 |
1967年 | 806篇 |
1966年 | 780篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
841.
A case study is presented of the treatment of an Arab coed by a Jewish trainee, and a phantom cotherapist using a modified dialectical therapeutic approach. Its effect on the treatment process and outcome, and on the therapist is discussed. 相似文献
842.
843.
Carmen J. Neyra Lillian M. Range William C. Goggin 《Journal of applied social psychology》1990,20(11):861-868
To see if suicide ideators responded differently than nonideators after success or failure experiences, 61 college students who were relatively high or low ideators participated in a computer task on which half got bogus success feedback, the other half got bogus failure feedback. Both groups then completed the Reasons for Living Inventory (RFL). Two (High versus Low Suicide) X 2 (Success or Failure) analyses indicated main effects for suicide on overall RFL and four of the six RFL subscales. Although there were no main effects for success versus failure, an interaction indicated that, in the failure condition, low suicidal subjects were significantly higher than high suicidal subjects in overall reasons for living. Results suggest that suicidal individuals may be vulnerable to failure, and may need interventions to help them avoid potential failure or to bolster their coping mechanisms. 相似文献
844.
John W. Mason Thomas R. Kosten Steven M. Southwick Earl L. Giller 《Journal of applied social psychology》1990,20(21):1822-1846
An overview is presented of a pilot psychoendocrine study of PTSD inpatients in comparison with several subgroups of schizophrenic and affective disorder patients. Using a hormonal profile including Cortisol, norepinephrine, epinephrine, testosterone, and thyroxine, it was found that the mean values for the PTSD group were at or near the extreme end of the range for every hormone measured, i.e., relatively low for Cortisol and high for the remaining hormones. The possible clinical meaning of these findings is considered in the light of prior psychoendocrine research on chronic stress. The hormonal alterations in PTSD appear to be persistent and suggest the possibility of being linked largely to traits or character structure, perhaps particularly to cognitive variables related to defense and coping mechanisms, as reviewed in detail for each hormonal system. There appears to be a potential for a fruitful union between the traumatic stress and psychoendocrine fields and some future strategies for developing and strengthening such a union are suggested. 相似文献
845.
Children in Grades 4 through 6 read concrete sentences that they were asked to learn. Half the subjects were instructed to construct images representing the sentence meanings. The remaining participants were provided no strategy instructions (control condition). Individual differences in short-term memory were more highly associated with performance in the imagery than in the control condition. There was a clear positive effect of imagery instructions on learning efficiency among students with relatively greater short-term memory; there was no difference in learning efficiency between the imagery and control conditions among students below the median for short-term memory performance. All relevant analyses supported the conclusion that imaginal coding of sentences that are read is demanding of short-term memory capacity. 相似文献
846.
The temporal characteristics of repetitive finger tapping by the left and right hands were examined in two experiments. In the first experiment, interresponse intervals (IRIs) were recorded while right-handed male subjects tapped in synchrony with an auditory timing pulse (the synchronization phase) and then attempted to maintain the same tapping rate without the timing pulses (the continuation phase). The left and right hands performed separately, at four different rates (interpulse intervals of 250, 500, 750, and 1500 ms). There was no asymmetry of the asynchronies of the timing pulses and the associated responses in the synchronization phase or of the IRIs in either phase, but there was an asymmetry of chronization phase or of the IRIs in either phase, but there was an asymmetry in the temporal dispersion of the responses in both phases. in the second experiment, right-handed males tapped separately with each hand at three different speeds: as quickly as possible, at a fast but steady rate, and at a slow rhythmical rate. The speed asymmetry present when tapping as quickly as possible (with the preferred hand tapping more quickly ) was reduced when tapping at the fast steady rate and was absent when tapping at the slow rhythmical rate. The temporal dispersion of the IRIs produced by the nonpreferred hand was greater than the temporal dispersion of those produced by the preferred hand in all speed conditions. These results show smaller temporal dispersion of tapping by the preferred hand in right-handed males under different conditions, including submaximal speeds at which both hands respond at the same rate. This suggests that the motor system controlling the preferred hand in right-handers had more precise timing of response output than that controlling the nonpreferred hand. 相似文献
847.
Monosynaptic Hoffman reflexes (H reflexes) were recorded from the soleus muscle during the response latency of a warned reaction time (RT) task that required plantarflexion of the foot. The task was done under four conditions of predictability of the response signal (RS), created by the factorial combination of foreperiod duration (1 and 4 s) and variability (fixed and variable). RT varied systematically with RS predictability and was facilitated in conditions that favored prediction of the RS. The response latency was divided into two successive phases by the onset of reflex augmentation: a premotor phase of constant reflex amplitude and a succeeding motor phase marked by progressively increasing reflex amplitude. Reflex augmentation during the motor phase was coupled more closely to the imminent movement than to the preceding signal to respond. The duration of the premotor phase was unaffected by RS predictability, but the duration of the motor phase (like RT) was shorter when the RS was more predictable. The maximum H reflex amplitude reached during the motor phase was greater when the RS was more predictable. The tonic level of H reflex amplitude during the premotor phase was greater in conditions that made prediction of the RS difficult. A second experiment showed that this difference was present throughout the foreperiod. These results suggest that conditions that favor prediction of the RS enhance motor preparation. changes in motor preparation (which affect RT) affect the processes underlying reflex amplitudes in the premotor phase and throughout the preceding foreperiod, in conditions that make prediction of the RS difficult, appear to reflect heightened general arousal. 相似文献
848.
Two experiments are reported, which examined the relative contributions of preload (resting force level), change of force, and the time taken to achieve the force in determining isometric force variability. The findings showed that change of force is the strongest determiner of peak force variability but that preload and time to peak force have smaller though systematic effects. A formula that predicts peak force variability is proposed, with preload as an additive effect to the ratio between the change of force level and the square root of time to peak force. These findings confirm that these three impulse variables are significant in predicting force variability and that the impact of rate of force on peak force variability is nonlinear. 相似文献
849.
G Chastain 《The Journal of general psychology》1990,117(3):295-301
The present study was conducted to determine whether sensitivity to a member of a pair of stimulus letters projecting to the parafovae would be improved by presenting that member at fixation. The effect was examined for the pair member appearing at a constant distance from fixation, which always was flanked to its inner or outer side. Contrary to expectations based upon results reported by Geiger and Lettvin (1986), observed accuracy changes were found to be due to response bias rather than to sensitivity enhancement. 相似文献
850.
M Meerum Terwogt 《The Journal of general psychology》1990,117(1):59-69
Two groups of normal and disordered children (6 to 7 years old and 10 to 11 years old) were presented with stories that could evoke the simultaneous experience of two different emotional impulses, especially when the two emotions were contrary in nature. Acknowledgment of the multiple emotional character of the situations occurred with the same frequency among normal and disordered children, but the disordered children more often denied all emotions or claimed that all negative emotions were applicable to the stories that contained two negative impulses. Disordered children also ascribed a higher intensity to acknowledged emotions than did normal children. It is argued that the acknowledgment of emotions is experienced as threatening by disordered children, which prevents them from analyzing the situation as thoroughly as normal children would. 相似文献