全文获取类型
收费全文 | 91606篇 |
免费 | 1530篇 |
国内免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
93142篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 1084篇 |
2019年 | 1326篇 |
2018年 | 1795篇 |
2017年 | 1894篇 |
2016年 | 1988篇 |
2015年 | 1393篇 |
2014年 | 1671篇 |
2013年 | 7563篇 |
2012年 | 3030篇 |
2011年 | 3080篇 |
2010年 | 1911篇 |
2009年 | 1918篇 |
2008年 | 2692篇 |
2007年 | 2687篇 |
2006年 | 2408篇 |
2005年 | 2070篇 |
2004年 | 2079篇 |
2003年 | 1964篇 |
2002年 | 1920篇 |
2001年 | 2814篇 |
2000年 | 2631篇 |
1999年 | 2026篇 |
1998年 | 981篇 |
1997年 | 882篇 |
1996年 | 929篇 |
1995年 | 883篇 |
1994年 | 842篇 |
1993年 | 844篇 |
1992年 | 1681篇 |
1991年 | 1534篇 |
1990年 | 1562篇 |
1989年 | 1448篇 |
1988年 | 1451篇 |
1987年 | 1354篇 |
1986年 | 1346篇 |
1985年 | 1362篇 |
1984年 | 1170篇 |
1983年 | 1051篇 |
1982年 | 811篇 |
1979年 | 1249篇 |
1978年 | 901篇 |
1975年 | 1002篇 |
1974年 | 1122篇 |
1973年 | 1164篇 |
1972年 | 951篇 |
1971年 | 904篇 |
1969年 | 816篇 |
1968年 | 1024篇 |
1967年 | 915篇 |
1966年 | 810篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
121.
122.
123.
Three mirror tracing experiments were conducted to investigate the connection between perception and motor behavior. In the first experiment, some subjects traced a hex-maze, other subjects traced a hex-maze after observing a model trace, others traced a hex-maze after reading instructions on mirror images, and others traced a hex-maze after having observed a model and heard the instructions. There were no significant differences between the groups' error scores, but their time scores differed significantly, although not always in the predicted direction. In Experiment 2, the subjects were to trace selected letters of the alphabet. Error scores for the second experiment did not differ much from those for the first experiment. In Experiment 3, the experimenter gave each subject commands for the correct directions of movement, using the subject's body as a frame-of-reference. There was little improvement in motor performance. These results suggest that the visual information presented in the mirror captured the subjects' attention and blocked their motor tracing program. 相似文献
124.
Associations of two different response sets on the Infrequent Response Index of the Strong Interest Inventory (SII) were examined. In one, participants responded to each item randomly; in the other, participants deliberately misrepresented their responses. The finding of scores higher than those proposed in the SII manual for the Infrequent Response Index suggests an alternative interpretation of this critical index. 相似文献
125.
126.
To integrate the 5-dimensional simple-structure and circumplex models of personality, the Abridged Big Five Dimensional Circumplex (AB5C) taxonomy of personality traits was developed, consisting of the 10 circumplexes that can be formed by pitting each of the Big Five factors against one another. The model maps facets of the Big Five dimensions as blends of 2 factors. An application to data consisting of 636 self-ratings and peer ratings on 540 personality trait adjectives yielded 34 well-defined facets out of a possible 45. The AB5C solution is compared with simple-structure and lower dimensional circumplex solutions, and its integrative and corrective potential are discussed, as well as its limitations. 相似文献
127.
R C Kessler K S Kendler A Heath M C Neale L J Eaves 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1992,62(2):257-272
A survey of 821 same-sex female twin pairs from a population-based registry assessed 8 dimensions of social support and social integration. Twin analyses documented significant common environmental influences on 5 of these 8 measures and significant genetic influences on 5 of the 8. A decomposition of the multiplicative association between support and a measure of stressful life experiences in predicting depressed mood--an association typically interpreted as providing evidence for a stress-buffering effect of social support--shows clearly that it is the environmental and genetic factors that cause support, rather than support itself, that buffer the effects of stress on mood in most cases. We discuss the implications of this result for future research on the relationship between social support and psychopathology. 相似文献
128.
Coordination of knowledge in communication: effects of speakers' assumptions about what others know.
Two pairs of studies examined effects of perspective taking in communication, using a 2-stage methodology that first obtained people's estimates of the recognizability to others of specific stimuli (public figures and everyday objects) and then examined the effects of these estimates on message formulation in a referential communication task. Ss were good at estimating stimulus identifiability but were biased in the direction of their own knowledge. The amount of information in a referring expression varied inversely with the perceived likelihood that addresses could identify the target stimulus. However, effects were less strong than anticipated. Although communicators do take others' knowledge into account, the extent to which they do so involves a trade-off with other sorts of information in the communicative situation. 相似文献
129.
Humans and monkeys show intermittent arm movements while tracking moving targets. This intermittency has been explained by postulating either a psychological refractory period after each movement and/or an error deadzone, an area surrounding the target within which movements are not initiated. We present a technique to detect and quantify the size of this deadzone, using a compensatory tracking paradigm that distinguishes it from a psychological refractory period. An artificial deadzone of variable size was added around a visual target displayed on a computer screen. While the subject was within this area, he received visual feedback that showed him to be directly on target. The presence of this artificial deadzone could affect tracking performance only if it exceeded the size of his intrinsic deadzone. Therefore, the size of artificial deadzone at which performance began to be affected revealed the size of the intrinsic deadzone. Measured at the subjects' eye, the deadzone was found to vary between 0.06 and 0.38 degrees, depending on the tracking task and viewing conditions; on the screen, this range was 1.3 mm to 3.3 mm. It increased with increasing speed of the target, with increasing viewing distance, and when the amplitude of the movement required was reduced. However, the deadzone size was not significantly correlated with the subjects' level of performance. We conclude that an intrinsic deadzone exists during compensatory tracking, and we suggest that its size is set by a cognitive process not simply related to the difficulty of the tracking task. 相似文献
130.
Affect, personality, and social activity. 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
D Watson L A Clark C W McIntyre S Hamaker 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1992,63(6):1011-1025
These studies examined relations between social activity and state and trait measures of Positive and Negative Affect. In Study 1 Ss completed scales relevant to 3-factor models of personality and a weekly mood and social activity questionnaire for 13 weeks. In Study 2 Ss completed measures of the 5-factor model of personality and a daily mood and social activity survey for 6-7 weeks. In within- and between-Ss analyses, socializing correlated significantly with state measures of Positive Affect and with trait measures of Extraversion/Positive Emotionality. These relations were relatively general across various types of positive affect and social events; however, specific types of social events also were differentially related to affect. In contrast, social activity had no consistent association with measures of Negative Affect or the other personality dimensions. The results support a temperamental view of Extraversion. 相似文献