全文获取类型
收费全文 | 14504篇 |
免费 | 300篇 |
国内免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
14831篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 1630篇 |
2011年 | 1803篇 |
2010年 | 406篇 |
2009年 | 169篇 |
2008年 | 1386篇 |
2007年 | 1372篇 |
2006年 | 1256篇 |
2005年 | 1008篇 |
2004年 | 898篇 |
2003年 | 850篇 |
2002年 | 777篇 |
2001年 | 582篇 |
2000年 | 679篇 |
1999年 | 274篇 |
1998年 | 70篇 |
1997年 | 55篇 |
1996年 | 40篇 |
1995年 | 38篇 |
1994年 | 34篇 |
1993年 | 48篇 |
1992年 | 36篇 |
1991年 | 36篇 |
1990年 | 31篇 |
1989年 | 21篇 |
1988年 | 36篇 |
1987年 | 31篇 |
1986年 | 30篇 |
1985年 | 35篇 |
1984年 | 40篇 |
1983年 | 35篇 |
1982年 | 22篇 |
1981年 | 38篇 |
1980年 | 25篇 |
1979年 | 38篇 |
1978年 | 31篇 |
1977年 | 21篇 |
1974年 | 19篇 |
1963年 | 19篇 |
1959年 | 41篇 |
1958年 | 101篇 |
1957年 | 104篇 |
1956年 | 91篇 |
1955年 | 76篇 |
1954年 | 79篇 |
1953年 | 69篇 |
1952年 | 60篇 |
1951年 | 43篇 |
1950年 | 33篇 |
1949年 | 29篇 |
1948年 | 32篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
81.
The movements of newborns have been thoroughly studied in terms of reflexes, muscle synergies, leg coordination, and target-directed
arm/hand movements. Since these approaches have concentrated mainly on separate accomplishments, there has remained a clear
need for more integrated investigations. Here, we report an inquiry in which we explicitly concentrated on taking such a perspective
and, additionally, were guided by the methodological concept of home base behavior, which Ilan Golani developed for studies
of exploratory behavior in animals. Methods from nonlinear dynamics, such as symbolic dynamics and recurrence plot analyses
of kinematic data received from audiovisual newborn recordings, yielded new insights into the spatial and temporal organization
of limb movements. In the framework of home base behavior, our approach uncovered a novel reference system of spontaneous
newborn movements. 相似文献
82.
Stams GJ Brugman D Deković M van Rosmalen L van der Laan P Gibbs JC 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2006,34(5):692-708
A meta-analysis of 50 studies was conducted to investigate whether juvenile delinquents use lower levels of moral judgment than their nondelinquent age-mates and, if so, what factors may influence or moderate the developmental delay. The results show a lower stage of moral judgment for juvenile delinquents (d=.76). Effect sizes were large for comparisons involving male offenders, late adolescents, delinquents with low intelligence, and incarcerated delinquents. The largest effect sizes were found for period of incarceration and comparisons involving juvenile delinquents with psychopathic disorder. Production instead of recognition measures, dilemma-free assessment methods, and non-blind scoring procedures yielded relatively large effect sizes, whereas effect sizes were medium for comparisons involving delinquents with average intelligence, non-incarcerated delinquents, female offenders, as well as early and middle adolescents. Psychopathic disorder and institutionalization were identified as unique moderators of the link between moral judgment and juvenile delinquency. It is concluded that developmentally delayed moral judgment is strongly associated with juvenile delinquency, even after controlling for socioeconomic status, gender, age and intelligence.This article stems from a paper presented at the meeting of the Association for Moral Education, July 2006, Fribourg, Switzerland. 相似文献
83.
Engineering ethics, individuals, and organizations 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Davis M 《Science and engineering ethics》2006,12(2):223-231
This article evaluates a family of criticism of how engineering ethics is now generally taught. The short version of the criticism
might be put this way: Teachers of engineering ethics devote too much time to individual decisions and not enough time to social context. There are at least six version of this criticism, each corresponding to a specific subject omitted. Teachers of engineering
ethics do not (it is said) teach enough about: 1) the culture of organizations; 2) the organization of organizations; 3) the
legal environment of organizations; 4) the role of professions in organizations; 5) the role of organizations in professions;
or 6) the political environment of organizations. My conclusion is that, while all six are worthy subjects, there is neither
much reason to believe that any of them are now absent from courses in engineering ethics nor an obvious way to decide whether
they (individually or in combination) are (or are not) now being given their due. What we have here is a dispute about how
much is enough. Such disputes are not to be settled without agreement concerning how we are to tell we have enough of this
or that. Right now we seem to lack that agreement—and not to have much reason to expect it any time soon.
An earlier version of this paper was presented at the 2005 conference, Ethics and Social Responsibility in Engineering and Technology, Linking Workplace Ethics and Education, co-hosted by Gonzaga University and Loyola Marymount University, Los Angeles, CA, USA, 9–10 June 2005. 相似文献
84.
We studied economic choice behavior in capuchin monkeys by offering them to choose between two different foods available in variable amounts. When monkeys selected between familiar foods, their choice patterns were well-described in terms of relative value of the two foods. A leading view in economics and biology is that such behavior results from stimulus-response associations acquired through experience. According to this view, values are not psychologically real; they can only be defined a posteriori. One prediction of this associative model is that animals faced for the first time with a new pair of foods learn to choose between them gradually. We tested this prediction. Surprisingly, we find that monkeys choose as effectively between new pairs of foods as they choose between familiar pairs of foods. We therefore, propose a cognitive model in which economic choice results from a two-stage mental process of value-assignment and decision-making. In a follow-up experiment, we find that the relative value assigned to three foods in sessions in which we tested them against each other combine according to transitivity. 相似文献
85.
We used a contextual priming paradigm to examine top-down influences on the face-inversion effect. Adult participants were primed with either faces or Chinese characters and then tested on ambiguous figures that could be perceived as either faces or Chinese characters, dependent on the priming condition. The ambiguous figures differed from one another in their configural information, which is crucial for processing faces but not Chinese characters. The inversion effect was observed in the face-priming condition, but not in the character-priming condition. The present results provide the first direct evidence that top-down activation of the face-processing expertise system plays a crucial role in the face-inversion effect. 相似文献
86.
Thewissen R Snijders SJ Havermans RC van den Hout M Jansen A 《Behaviour research and therapy》2006,44(10):1441-1449
The effects of cue exposure therapy are limited, because renewal after extinction is an important source of relapse. In this study, 33 smokers were exposed to a cue predicting smoking availability and a cue predicting smoking unavailability in one context (acquisition context A). Following extinction in another context (extinction context B), a test for renewal took place in the original acquisition context A (i.e. ABA renewal). Urge to smoke was measured using a Visual Analogue Scale. Renewal of differential conditioned urge responding occurred when participants were tested in the acquisition context, while differential urge responding remained extinguished when tested in the extinction context. This experiment provides evidence that ABA renewal occurred in smokers. Clinical implications are discussed. 相似文献
87.
The “prawn-in-the-tube” procedure in the cuttlefish: Habituation or passive avoidance learning?
下载免费PDF全文

Agin V Chichery R Dickel L Chichery MP 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2006,13(1):97-101
This study examines whether or not habituation contributes to the regulation of the inhibition of predatory behavior observed during the "prawn-in-the-tube" training procedure. When presented with prawns that are visible behind glass but untouchable, cuttlefish promptly learn to inhibit their capture attempts. The first three experiments demonstrated that the acquired response in the course of training cannot be dishabituated. The fourth experiment demonstrated that the repeated application of a brief visual prawn stimulus, one that is terminated before the cuttlefish can strike, decreases attack latencies. Taken together, the results of this study establish that habituation does not play a significant role in this learning task. In fact, the present findings strengthen the results of previous studies suggesting that passive avoidance learning contributes to the regulation of the inhibition of predatory behavior. 相似文献
88.
89.
Recent studies have shown that emotionally significant stimuli are often better identified than neutral stimuli. It is not clear, however, whether these results are due to enhanced perceptual processing or to a bias favoring the identification of emotionally significant stimuli over neutral stimuli. The present study used a two-alternative forced-choice perceptual identification task to disentangle the effects of bias and enhanced processing. We found that emotionally significant targets were better identified than neutral targets. In contrast, the emotional significance of the foil alternative had no effect on performance. The present results support the hypothesis that perceptual encoding of emotionally significant stimuli is enhanced. 相似文献
90.
Norman JF Crabtree CE Herrmann M Thompson SR Shular CF Clayton AM 《Perception & psychophysics》2006,68(1):94-101
In two experiments, we investigated the ability of younger and older observers to perceive and discriminate 3-D shape from static and dynamic patterns of binocular disparity. In both experiments, the younger observers' discrimination accuracies were 20% higher than those of the older observers. Despite this quantitative difference, in all other respects the older observers performed similarly to the younger observers. Both age groups were similarly affected by changes in the magnitude of binocular disparity, by reductions in binocular correspondence, and by increases in the speed of stereoscopic motion. In addition, observers in both age groups exhibited an advantage in performance for dynamic stereograms when the patterns of binocular disparity contained significant amounts of correspondence "noise." The process of aging does affect stereopsis, but the effects are quantitative rather than qualitative. 相似文献