全文获取类型
收费全文 | 10544篇 |
免费 | 232篇 |
国内免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
10792篇 |
出版年
2012年 | 1216篇 |
2011年 | 1287篇 |
2010年 | 304篇 |
2009年 | 121篇 |
2008年 | 1027篇 |
2007年 | 1027篇 |
2006年 | 933篇 |
2005年 | 782篇 |
2004年 | 649篇 |
2003年 | 615篇 |
2002年 | 550篇 |
2001年 | 422篇 |
2000年 | 494篇 |
1999年 | 188篇 |
1998年 | 48篇 |
1997年 | 45篇 |
1996年 | 30篇 |
1995年 | 24篇 |
1994年 | 26篇 |
1993年 | 40篇 |
1992年 | 28篇 |
1991年 | 24篇 |
1990年 | 25篇 |
1989年 | 16篇 |
1988年 | 24篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1986年 | 26篇 |
1985年 | 25篇 |
1984年 | 25篇 |
1983年 | 24篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 23篇 |
1980年 | 18篇 |
1979年 | 25篇 |
1978年 | 22篇 |
1977年 | 13篇 |
1974年 | 13篇 |
1963年 | 13篇 |
1959年 | 26篇 |
1958年 | 69篇 |
1957年 | 64篇 |
1956年 | 62篇 |
1955年 | 46篇 |
1954年 | 46篇 |
1953年 | 49篇 |
1952年 | 39篇 |
1951年 | 24篇 |
1950年 | 23篇 |
1949年 | 18篇 |
1948年 | 19篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
21.
22.
The effects of modifying consummatory behavior on the topography of the autoshaped pecking response in pigeons 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

The autoshaped responses of two debeaked pigeons that had developed modified eating behavior were compared to the autoshaped responses of three control subjects that ate grain normally. The control subjects exhibited keylight responding that was judged normal by two independent raters. The debeaked subjects pecked the key and ate grain with both normal and modified responses. The results of this study demostrate that an autoshaping procedure using grain as an appetitive stimulus may be used to establish a response that is not biologically preorganized. 相似文献
23.
24.
Davison M 《Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior》1991,55(2):189-200
Five pigeons were trained on concurrent variable-interval schedules arranged on two keys. In Part 1 of the experiment, the subjects responded under no constraints, and the ratios of reinforcers obtainable were varied over five levels. In Part 2, the conditions of the experiment were changed such that the time spent responding on the left key before a subsequent changeover to the right key determined the minimum time that must be spent responding on the right key before a changeover to the left key could occur. When the left key provided a higher reinforcer rate than the right key, this procedure ensured that the time allocated to the two keys was approximately equal. The data showed that such a time-allocation constraint only marginally constrained response allocation. In Part 3, the numbers of responses emitted on the left key before a changeover to the right key determined the minimum number of responses that had to be emitted on the right key before a changeover to the left key could occur. This response constraint completely constrained time allocation. These data are consistent with the view that response allocation is a fundamental process (and time allocation a derivative process), or that response and time allocation are independently controlled, in concurrent-schedule performance. 相似文献
25.
Critics have questioned the value of human operant conditioning experiments in the study of fundamental processes of reinforcement. Contradictory results from human and animal experiments have been attributed to the complex social and verbal history of the human subject. On these grounds, it has been contended that procedures that mimic those conventionally used with animal subjects represent a "poor analytic preparation" for the explication of reinforcement principles. In defending the use of conventional operant methods for human research, we make three points: (a) Historical variables play a critical role in research on processes of reinforcement, regardless of whether the subjects are humans or animals. (b) Techniques are available for detecting, analyzing, and counteracting such historical and extra-experimental influences; these include long-term observations, steady state designs, and, when variables are not amenable to direct control (e.g., age, gender, species), selection of subjects with common characteristics. (c) Other forms of evidence that might be used to validate conditioning principles-applied behavior analysis and behavioristic interpretation-have inherent limitations and cannot substitute for experimental analysis. We conclude that human operant conditioning experiments are essential for the analysis of the reinforcement process at the human level, but caution that their value depends on the extent to which the traditional methods of the experimental analysis of behavior are properly applied. 相似文献
26.
一般地说,认知风格可以被理解为个人感知、组织以及评价信息的独特方式。然而,除此之外,本文要论及的四项系列研究运用了两种量表,它们反映了对认知风格的不同而独特的解释。一种量表是迈尔斯—布立格斯类型量表(Myers—Briggs Type lndlcator),这是一套著名的量表,它是以荣格(Jungian)的人格理 相似文献
27.
28.
Time-series analysis in operant research 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
A time-series method is presented, nontechnically, for analysis of data generated in individual-subject operant studies, and is recommended as a supplement to visual analysis of behavior change in reversal or multiple-baseline experiments. The method can be used to identify three kinds of statistically significant behavior change: (a) changes in score levels from one experimental phase to another, (b) reliable upward or downward trends in scores, and (c) changes in trends between phases. The detection of, and reliance on, serial dependency (autocorrelation among temporally adjacent scores) in individual-subject behavioral scores is emphasized. Examples of published data from the operant literature are used to illustrate the time-series method. 相似文献
29.
The idea of viewing human cognition as a rational solution to computational problems posed by the environment has influenced several recent theories of human memory. The first rational models of memory demonstrated that human memory seems to be remarkably well adapted to environmental statistics but made only minimal assumptions about the form of the environmental information represented in memory. Recently, several probabilistic methods for representing the latent semantic structure of language have been developed, drawing on research in computer science, statistics and computational linguistics. These methods provide a means of extending rational models of memory retrieval to linguistic stimuli, and a way to explore the influence of the statistics of language on human memory. 相似文献
30.
In this article, the authors present two laboratory experiments testing a group-level perspective on the role of empathy in helping. Experiment 1 tested the authors' predictions in an intercultural context of helping. Confirming their specific Empathy x Group Membership moderation hypothesis, empathy had a stronger effect on helping intentions when the helper and the target belonged to the same cultural group than when they belonged to different groups. Experiment 2 replicated these findings in a modified minimal group paradigm using laboratory-created groups. Moreover, this second experiment also provides evidence for the hypothesized psychological mechanisms underlying the empathy-(ingroup) helping relationship. Specifically, analyses in the ingroup condition confirmed that the strength of the empathy-(ingroup) helping relationship systematically varied as a function of perceived similarities among ingroup members. The general implications of these findings for empathy-motivated helping are discussed. 相似文献