全文获取类型
收费全文 | 378篇 |
免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
380篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 2篇 |
2021年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 4篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 9篇 |
2016年 | 17篇 |
2015年 | 4篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 50篇 |
2012年 | 12篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 7篇 |
2008年 | 17篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 22篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 12篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 17篇 |
2001年 | 8篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 5篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 4篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1988年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 7篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 6篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有380条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
301.
This paper examines the use of ‘burden' metaphors relating to the experience of depression in one good and one poor outcome process-experiential short-term psychotherapy dyad. Burden-metaphors are meaningful as they appear to characterize the way these clients experience the course of their depressions (e.g. Korman & Angus, 1995). Understanding how these experiences are dealt with in productive therapies, as compared to less-productive treatments, can aid in therapist training and in developing therapeutic programmes for depressed clients. The Experiencing Scale (Klein et al., 1970) and the Narrative Process Coding System (Angus et al., 1996) are used to examine the processes at play when burden-metaphors are used in the good and the poor therapy. Results indicate that, in the good outcome dyad, metaphors of ‘being burdened' were transformed into metaphors of ‘unloading the burden' over the course of the therapy, while there was no transformation evident in the poor-outcome dyad. The good outcome therapy tended to have a higher level of experiencing when discussing burden-metaphors, in comparison with the poor-outcome therapy. Furthermore, the successful dyad tended to more often use internal narrative sequences (as identified by the NPCS) in the exploraiton of metaphoric expression. 相似文献
302.
In two experiments, third, fifth, and eleventh graders attributed males' and females' successes and failures on sex-stereotypic tasks to (a) task difficulty, (b) effort, (c) luck, or (d) skill. Male and female children responded similarly within and across age. Attributions for successes varied with congruity of actor and task stereotypes; effort was emphasized for sex-inconsistent relative to sex-consistent performance. However, over-riding sex-of-actor biases emerged in explanations for failures, suggesting failures were treated as anticipated outcomes for females. Results are considered in terms of maintenance of sex-role stereotypes beginning early in childhood.A portion of the first experiment reported in this article was presented at the annual meeting of the Eastern Psychological Association, Washington, D. C., March 1978. 相似文献
303.
Lynne E. Olson 《Science and engineering ethics》2010,16(1):185-200
Education in the responsible conduct of research (RCR) in the United States has evolved over the past decade from targeting
trainees to including educational efforts aimed at faculty and staff. In addition RCR education has become more focused as
federal agencies have moved to recommend specific content and to mandate education in certain areas. RCR education has therefore
become a research-compliance issue necessitating the development of policies and the commitment of resources to develop or
expand systems for educating faculty and staff and for assuring compliance. These changes implied the need to develop a program
evaluation model that could be applied to institutional RCR education programs, which were expected to differ from traditional
academic credit-bearing courses targeting trainees. Information gleaned from the examination of corporate compliance models
was analyzed in order to create a program evaluation module that could be used to document and assess educational programs
focused on teaching RCR. A programmed series of questions for each of the nine RCR content areas identified by the United
States Office of Research Integrity was created based on a performance-monitoring evaluation model. The questions focus on
educational goals, resources provided to support the educational efforts, educational content, content delivery, educational
outcomes, compliance requirements and feedback. Answers collected in response to the questions could be used to both document
and continually improve the quality of RCR educational programs through on-going formative assessment and feedback. 相似文献
304.
In these studies, we examined the role of elaborations for subjects learning a procedural skill (viz., using a personal computer) from an instructional text. In Experiment 1, we compared two sources of elaborations: those provided by the author and those generated by learners while reading. In the latter condition, subjects were given advance information about the tasks they were to perform so that they would generate more specific task-related elaborations while reading. Each source of elaborations facilitated skill performance. This result contrasts with results of the past experiments testing declarative knowledge in which author-provided elaborations were found to hurt performance. In Experiment 2, the author-provided elaborations were classified into those illustrating the syntax of the operating system commands and those explaining basic concepts and their applicability. Syntax elaborations produced significant facilitation for experienced and novice computer users. Concept elaborations produced no reliable improvement. 相似文献
305.
306.
Simon Lacey Yaseen Jamal Sara M. List Kelly McCormick K. Sathian Lynne C. Nygaard 《Cognitive Science》2020,44(9):e12883
Sound symbolism refers to non-arbitrary mappings between the sounds of words and their meanings and is often studied by pairing auditory pseudowords such as “maluma” and “takete” with rounded and pointed visual shapes, respectively. However, it is unclear what auditory properties of pseudowords contribute to their perception as rounded or pointed. Here, we compared perceptual ratings of the roundedness/pointedness of large sets of pseudowords and shapes to their acoustic and visual properties using a novel application of representational similarity analysis (RSA). Representational dissimilarity matrices (RDMs) of the auditory and visual ratings of roundedness/pointedness were significantly correlated crossmodally. The auditory perceptual RDM correlated significantly with RDMs of spectral tilt, the temporal fast Fourier transform (FFT), and the speech envelope. Conventional correlational analyses showed that ratings of pseudowords transitioned from rounded to pointed as vocal roughness (as measured by the harmonics-to-noise ratio, pulse number, fraction of unvoiced frames, mean autocorrelation, shimmer, and jitter) increased. The visual perceptual RDM correlated significantly with RDMs of global indices of visual shape (the simple matching coefficient, image silhouette, image outlines, and Jaccard distance). Crossmodally, the RDMs of the auditory spectral parameters correlated weakly but significantly with those of the global indices of visual shape. Our work establishes the utility of RSA for analysis of large stimulus sets and offers novel insights into the stimulus parameters underlying sound symbolism, showing that sound-to-shape mapping is driven by acoustic properties of pseudowords and suggesting audiovisual cross-modal correspondence as a basis for language users' sensitivity to this type of sound symbolism. 相似文献
307.
Lynne Baker-Ward Caroline Staneck Tyler Jennifer L. Coffman Kathy A. Merritt Peter A. Ornstein 《Applied cognitive psychology》2020,34(2):330-342
This investigation extended work on the linkage between knowledge and remembering by exploring the relation between generic and episodic memory representations. Thirty 6-year-old children experienced a mock physical examination with some expected components omitted and other unexpected actions included. Immediately and again after 12 weeks, the children reported the event, answered questions about what usually happens in an examination, and rated their confidence in aspects of their reports. They remembered more typical than atypical present components, that is, those included in the examination, and, over time, falsely reported more typical than atypical actions. The children produced intrusions of expected-but-omitted medical features at the delay but with lower confidence ratings than they provided for correctly recalled items. Performance on a source monitoring task was associated with aspects of the children's confidence ratings for intrusions. The findings provide evidence that the relation between episodic and generic representations is dynamic and suggest that the capacity to differentiate between them contributes to the development of accurate eyewitness memory. 相似文献
308.
Trevor G. Mazzucchelli Emma Burton Lynne Roberts 《Australian journal of psychology》2019,71(3):305-311
309.
The current study assessed the extent to which the use of referential prosody varies with communicative demand. Speaker–listener dyads completed a referential communication task during which speakers attempted to indicate one of two color swatches (one bright, one dark) to listeners. Speakers' bright sentences were reliably higher pitched than dark sentences for ambiguous (e.g., bright red versus dark red) but not unambiguous (e.g., bright red versus dark purple) trials, suggesting that speakers produced meaningful acoustic cues to brightness when the accompanying linguistic content was underspecified (e.g., “Can you get the red one?”). Listening partners reliably chose the correct corresponding swatch for ambiguous trials when lexical information was insufficient to identify the target, suggesting that listeners recruited prosody to resolve lexical ambiguity. Prosody can thus be conceptualized as a type of vocal gesture that can be recruited to resolve referential ambiguity when there is communicative demand to do so. 相似文献
310.
Leonard A. Jason Renee Taylor Lynne Wagner Jay Holden Joseph R. Ferrari Audrius V. Plioplys Sigita Plioplys David Lipkin Morris Papernik 《American journal of community psychology》1995,23(4):557-568
Most of the Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS) epidemiological studies have relied on physicians who refer patients having at
least 6 months of chronic fatigue and other symptoms. However, there are a number of potential problems when using this method
to derive prevalence statistics. For example, some individuals with CFS might not have the economic resources to access medical
care. Other individuals with CFS might be reluctant to use medical personnel, particularly if they have encountered physicians
skeptical of the authenticity of their illness. In addition, physicians that are skeptical of the existence of CFS might not
identify cases. In the present pilot study, a random community sample (N=1,031) was interviewed by telephone in order to identify and comprehensively evaluate individuals with symptoms of CFS and
those who self-report having CFS. Different definitions of CFS were employed, and higher rates (0.2%) of CFS were found than
in previous studies. Methodological benefits in using more rigorous epidemiological methods when estimating CFS prevalence
rates are discussed.
The authors appreciate the generous financial support of the CFIDS Association and Minnan, Inc. We also are grateful for many
helpful suggestions provided by Judith A. Richman, William McCready, Wendell Richmond, and Stephen E. Goldston. Finally, we
are very thankful to the many undergraduate volunteers who helped us complete this study, and they include Cheryl Stenzel,
Georgina DeLa Torre, Vickie Chrisos, Don Banik, Hillary Loeb, Leslie Ramesack, Ellen Diamond, Rob Bleeker, Gib Garza III,
Mindy Silverstein, Kadip Sen, Meredith Lombrazo, Brian Miller, Caroline Marsden, Lorraine Whitmore, Stan Gayot, Karen Costakis,
and Dave Sobotka. 相似文献