全文获取类型
收费全文 | 3399篇 |
免费 | 41篇 |
专业分类
3440篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 43篇 |
2023年 | 44篇 |
2022年 | 65篇 |
2021年 | 91篇 |
2020年 | 95篇 |
2019年 | 127篇 |
2018年 | 159篇 |
2017年 | 163篇 |
2016年 | 129篇 |
2015年 | 96篇 |
2014年 | 102篇 |
2013年 | 384篇 |
2012年 | 238篇 |
2011年 | 224篇 |
2010年 | 173篇 |
2009年 | 96篇 |
2008年 | 202篇 |
2007年 | 155篇 |
2006年 | 147篇 |
2005年 | 102篇 |
2004年 | 80篇 |
2003年 | 66篇 |
2002年 | 55篇 |
2001年 | 32篇 |
2000年 | 43篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 14篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1989年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 10篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 8篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1972年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 10篇 |
1969年 | 7篇 |
1968年 | 12篇 |
1963年 | 6篇 |
排序方式: 共有3440条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
61.
Juhani Järvikivi Roger P. G. van Gompel Jukka Hyönä 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》2017,46(3):525-550
Two visual-world eye-tracking experiments investigating pronoun resolution in Finnish examined the time course of implicit causality information relative to both grammatical role and order-of-mention information. Experiment 1 showed an effect of implicit causality that appeared at the same time as the first-mention preference. Furthermore, when we counterbalanced the semantic roles of the verbs, we found no effect of grammatical role, suggesting the standard observed subject preference has a large semantic component. Experiment 2 showed that both the personal pronoun hän and the demonstrative tämä preferred the antecedent consistent with the implicit causality bias; tämä was not interpreted as referring to the semantically non-prominent entity. In contrast, structural prominence affected hän and tämä differently: we found a first-mention preference for hän, but a second-mention preference for tämä. The results suggest that semantic implicit causality information has an immediate effect on pronoun resolution and its use is not delayed relative to order-of-mention information. Furthermore, they show that order-of-mention differentially affects different types of anaphoric expressions, but semantic information has the same effect. 相似文献
62.
63.
64.
Pollatsek A Hyönä J Bertram R 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2000,26(2):820-833
The processing of transparent Finnish compound words was investigated in 2 experiments in which eye movements were recorded while sentences were read silently. The frequency of the second constituent had a large influence (95 ms) on gaze duration on the target words, but its influence was relatively late in processing: A clear effect only occurred on the probability of a third fixation. The frequency of the whole compound word had a similar influence on gaze duration (82 ms) and influenced eye movements at least as rapidly as did the frequency of the second constituent. These results, together with an earlier finding that the frequency of the first constituent affected the first fixation duration, indicate that the identification of these compound words involves parallel processing of both morphological constituents and whole-word representations. 相似文献
65.
Philosophical Studies - 相似文献
66.
The paper presents a journey along the history of phenomenology (Husserlian Phenomenology or phenomenology derived from Husserl) in Mexico, dealing mainly with its appropriation in the field of philosophy proper. The survey covers almost the whole century, leaving out only the last years and the present situation. Although all those philosophers who came in touch with phenomenology in a significant way in that period are mentioned here, particular attention is laid upon three figures and their meaning in Mexican philosophical culture: they are Antonio Caso, the first influential thinker and professor to dedicate a book to Husserl's philosophy; José Gaos, who enjoys a well-deserved fame as teacher and translator of phenomenological works, and Jorge Portilla, the author of a most valuable phenomenological essay: Phenomenology of Relajo. Thus, in these three cases, their contributions are explored in a somewhat more detailed way. 相似文献
67.
Kuzeymen Balıkçı Orkun Aydın İpek Sönmez Bengü Kalo Pınar Ünal-Aydın 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2020,61(5):593-598
Social networking sites (SNSs) enrol new subscribers each day. However, problematic SNS use has undesirable effects on psychological functioning. Therefore, it is important to identify the factors that contribute to the development of problematic SNS use. Very few studies have focused on revealing the underlying mechanisms of problematic SNS use. Although many past studies have examined the relationship between metacognitive beliefs and Internet addiction, the association between metacognitive beliefs and problematic SNS use has not been adequately explored. In this study, we aimed to explore the association between metacognitive beliefs and problematic SNS use among young adults. A total of 308 individuals participated in this study. A socio-demographic data form, the Metacognitions Questionnaire-30 (MCQ-30), and Social Media Addiction Scale (SMAS) were administered. Group comparisons were performed using multivariate analysis of covariance. Pearson's correlational and multiple linear regression analyses were conducted to examine the associations between metacognitive beliefs and problematic SNS use. The SNS addicts scored higher in all of the SMAS assessments. When compared to non-addicts, SNS addicts obtained higher scores on all the subtests of the SMAS and MCQ-30 except cognitive self-consciousness. The negative beliefs about the uncontrollability and danger of worry, cognitive confidence, and need for control thoughts were associated with SMAS mood modification, relapse and conflict subdimensions. Our findings revealed that dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs are related to problematic SNS use among young adults. These findings indicate that mental health workers should consider the modification of metacognitive beliefs in the treatment of problematic SNS use. 相似文献
68.
The aim of this study was to examine the relationships between cybervictimization and three body-related concerns: body-esteem, self-objectification, and internalization of body ideals. The aim was also to examine these relationships not only to cybervictimization in general but also to appearance-related cybervictimization more specifically. The sample comprised 482 adolescents (233 girls and 247 boys aged 13–15; two participants did not answer the question about gender) from four Swedish schools. The results showed that victims of appearance-related cyberbullying suffered from more body-related concerns: they had a poorer view of their general appearance and of their weight. They also reported more body shame, thin-ideal internalization, and appearance-related pressure from the media. This study shows that cyberbullying that focuses on the victim's appearance is associated with several body-related concerns that have not previously been studied. A novel finding was also that body-related concerns were not related to cyberbullying in general, as has been implied in earlier research, but specifically related to cyberbullying directed at the victim's appearance. The findings suggest that there is a need to include attention to the specific relationships between appearance-related cyberbullying and body-related concerns in future prevention and intervention work. 相似文献
69.
In this article we identify three previously unnoticed problems with flexible moral theories, i.e., theories according to which different moral rules apply when there is full compliance and when there is partial compliance. The first problem is that flexible theories are necessarily very complex, which undermines their ability to motivate and guide action. The second problem is that flexible theories allow for a troubling kind of (moral) domination: the duties an agent has depend on other agents' willingness to comply. Finally, flexible theories introduce indeterminacy: it is sometimes impossible to determine which moral rules apply. When agents stand at the threshold—i.e., when a different rule would apply if a single additional act of noncompliance took place—it is impossible to determine which rule should apply. 相似文献
70.