全文获取类型
收费全文 | 713篇 |
免费 | 10篇 |
专业分类
723篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 7篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 18篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 27篇 |
2016年 | 31篇 |
2015年 | 15篇 |
2014年 | 25篇 |
2013年 | 68篇 |
2012年 | 31篇 |
2011年 | 38篇 |
2010年 | 18篇 |
2009年 | 27篇 |
2008年 | 29篇 |
2007年 | 25篇 |
2006年 | 8篇 |
2005年 | 25篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 11篇 |
2002年 | 13篇 |
2001年 | 7篇 |
2000年 | 12篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 9篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 5篇 |
1979年 | 14篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 4篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 6篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 4篇 |
1965年 | 4篇 |
1951年 | 5篇 |
1923年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有723条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
301.
von Helversen B Wilke A Johnson T Schmid G Klapp B 《Journal of abnormal psychology》2011,120(4):962-968
Previous research reported conflicting results concerning the influence of depression on cognitive task performance. Whereas some studies reported that depression enhances performance, other studies reported negative or null effects. These discrepant findings appear to result from task variation, as well as the severity and treatment status of participant depression. To better understand these moderating factors, we study the performance of individuals-in a complex sequential decision task similar to the secretary problem-who are nondepressed, depressed, and recovering from a major depressive episode. We find that depressed individuals perform better than do nondepressed individuals. Formal modeling of participants' decision strategies suggested that acutely depressed participants had higher thresholds for accepting options and made better choices than either healthy participants or those recovering from depression. 相似文献
302.
S von Kluge F R Brush 《Journal of comparative psychology (Washington, D.C. : 1983)》1992,106(3):248-253
Syracuse high- and low-avoidance Long-Evans rats (Rattus norvegicus; SHA/Bru and SLA/Bru) were selectively bred for good and poor active-avoidance learning. However, SLA/Bru animals are superior to SHA/Bru rats in conditioned suppression and passive avoidance learning. In this experiment, saccharin taste and almond odor were the components of a compound conditioned stimulus (flavor) in an illness-induced aversive conditioning paradigm. SLA/Bru rats (n = 17) showed stronger conditioned flavor, taste, and odor aversion than did SHA/Bru animals (n = 18). Unselected Long-Evans rats (n = 18) were intermediate between the selected strains. SLA/Bru and Long-Evans rats showed taste-potentiated odor aversions in this experiment, whereas SHA/Bru animals did not. The results provide evidence that genetic factors, as exemplified by the different strains, are importantly involved in the mechanisms underlying interoceptive and exteroceptive aversive conditioning. 相似文献
303.
Ullsperger M Volz KG von Cramon DY 《Trends in cognitive sciences》2004,8(10):445-6; author reply 446-7
304.
Walter Erich Manfred von Lucadou 《Axiomathes》2011,21(2):263-285
A systemic phenomenological model that assumes the movability of the Cartesian cut is proposed and elucidated by means of
a single exploratory case study. The model assumes that a continuum from purely psychosomatic disorders to RSPK cases exists.
The degree of externalization (locus of control) of the affected person serves as an ordering parameter for the location of
the Cartesian cut. It turns out that the dynamics of the disorder develops in four phases, like in the RSPK-model of the MPI.
However, in contrast to the decline phase of the RSPK cases (because of the NT-axiom), CER cases lead to petrification of
the complaints. 相似文献
305.
306.
Within the last three decades western psychology, psychotherapy and medicine have become increasingly enamored by the perception of “mindfulness” as a form of unbiased, judgment-free awareness of perceptible experience. Derived from earliest forms of Buddhist psychology, mindfulness is, however, also integral to a particular set of ethical values and may be seen as a practice or process that tangibly embodies the Buddhist ethical stance. Buddhist psychology, similar to psychodynamic and other psychotherapeutic approaches, aims at the understanding and alleviation of human suffering. We believe that the intentional practice of mindfulness (as defined above) adds a new (very old) dimension by implicit cultivation of a consistent set of ethical attitudes that include kindness, patience, tolerance, generosity, compassion and courage. Sustained awareness of the spectrum of momentary experience may, otherwise, be inaccessible. These benevolent attitudinal qualities often allow eudemonic mental and physical states to be experienced, even in the face of unpleasant circumstances. A relational process unfolds that reinforces, in both bodily and mental domains, a synergy of awareness and ethical intentions literally representing an embodied ethical presence. We discuss the implications of such an understanding of mindfulness for current psychotherapeutic approaches. 相似文献
307.
Prof. Dr. Jörn von Wietersheim Jörg Oster Doro Munz Nicola Epple Edit Rottler Kathrin Mörtl 《Psychotherapeut》2008,53(6):424-431
Literature research and own clinical experiences confirm the significance of a combined use of settings and methods in the area of inpatient and outpatient psychotherapy. For inpatient treatment, the combined use of methods, such as individual psychotherapy, group therapy, creative therapies, and sport, are an essential part of the treatment. In outpatient treatment, the regulations of the German insurance do not permit such combinations. Nevertheless, they occur and are often proactively initiated by the patients. The various combined use of psychotherapy and psychotropics is very well studied, but also the combination of settings, i.e. supplementary inpatient treatment, day treatment, and outpatient treatment have to be considered. While administering their therapies, psychotherapists should keep in mind possibly concurrently ongoing treatments as well as former therapies and their effects on the patients. With the exception of combinations with psychotropic treatment, there exist almost no empirical data in this important field. 相似文献
308.
Severity of dysfluency correlates with basal ganglia activity in persistent developmental stuttering
Giraud AL Neumann K Bachoud-Levi AC von Gudenberg AW Euler HA Lanfermann H Preibisch C 《Brain and language》2008,104(2):190-199
Previous studies suggest that anatomical anomalies [Foundas, A. L., Bollich, A. M., Corey, D. M., Hurley, M., & Heilman, K. M. (2001). Anomalous anatomy of speech-language areas in adults with persistent developmental stuttering. Neurology, 57, 207-215; Foundas, A. L., Corey, D. M., Angeles, V., Bollich, A. M., Crabtree-Hartman, E., & Heilman, K. M. (2003). Atypical cerebral laterality in adults with persistent developmental stuttering. Neurology, 61, 1378-1385; Foundas, A. L., Bollich, A. M., Feldman, J., Corey, D. M., Hurley, M., & Lemen, L. C. et al., (2004). Aberrant auditory processing and atypical planum temporale in developmental stuttering. Neurology, 63, 1640-1646; Jancke, L., Hanggi, J., & Steinmetz, H. (2004). Morphological brain differences between adult stutterers and non-stutterers. BMC Neurology, 4, 23], in particular a reduction of the white matter anisotropy underlying the left sensorimotor cortex [Sommer, M., Koch, M. A., Paulus, W., Weiller, C., & Buchel, C. (2002). Disconnection of speech-relevant brain areas in persistent developmental stuttering. Lancet, 360, 380-383] could be at the origin of persistent developmental stuttering (PDS). Because neural connections between the motor cortex and basal ganglia are implicated in speech motor functions, PDS could also be associated with a dysfunction in basal ganglia activity [Alm, P. (2004). Stuttering and the basal ganglia circuits: a critical review of possible relations. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37, 325-369]. This fMRI study reports a correlation between severity of stuttering and activity in the basal ganglia and shows that this activity is modified by fluency shaping therapy through long-term therapy effects that reflect speech production improvement. A model of dysfunction in stuttering and possible repair modes is proposed that accommodates the data presented here and observations previously made by us and by others. 相似文献
309.
Three experiments examined the hypothesis that people show consistency in motivated social cognitive processing across self-serving domains. Consistent with this hypothesis, Experiment 1 revealed that people who rated a task at which they succeeded as more important than a task at which they failed also cheated on a series of math problems, but only when they could rationalize their cheating as unintentional. Experiment 2 replicated this finding and demonstrated that a self-report measure of self-deception did not predict this rationalized cheating. Experiment 3 replicated Experiments 1 and 2 and ruled out several alternative explanations. These experiments suggest that people who show motivated processing in ego-protective domains also show motivated processing in extrinsic domains. These experiments also introduce a new measurement procedure for differentiating between intentional versus rationalized cheating. 相似文献
310.
Anita von Raffay 《The Journal of analytical psychology》2000,45(4):573-575