首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   18413篇
  免费   868篇
  国内免费   8篇
  19289篇
  2020年   229篇
  2019年   233篇
  2018年   349篇
  2017年   336篇
  2016年   394篇
  2015年   307篇
  2014年   377篇
  2013年   1578篇
  2012年   635篇
  2011年   554篇
  2010年   382篇
  2009年   379篇
  2008年   495篇
  2007年   507篇
  2006年   435篇
  2005年   403篇
  2004年   370篇
  2003年   320篇
  2002年   347篇
  2001年   702篇
  2000年   690篇
  1999年   488篇
  1998年   199篇
  1997年   193篇
  1996年   191篇
  1992年   393篇
  1991年   365篇
  1990年   377篇
  1989年   323篇
  1988年   314篇
  1987年   304篇
  1986年   284篇
  1985年   279篇
  1984年   272篇
  1983年   233篇
  1982年   186篇
  1981年   165篇
  1979年   258篇
  1978年   195篇
  1977年   168篇
  1975年   208篇
  1974年   260篇
  1973年   248篇
  1972年   204篇
  1971年   201篇
  1970年   163篇
  1969年   195篇
  1968年   226篇
  1967年   213篇
  1966年   184篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
982.
Thirty-three twelve-month-old infants were divided into three groups based on the frequency and length of naturally-occurring nontraumatic separations from mother during the first year. One group had experienced only brief separations when left with a babysitter for an evening. A second group had been left once or twice for an average of five days while parents vacationed. A third group had been left regularly and frequently during the daytime, usually because mother was working. These groups were compared on measures of reaction to separation and reunion, in the home and in the laboratory, at one year of age. No group differences were found on any of the dependent variables. It is suggested that the quality of substitute care is probably more important than separation per se in mediating the impact of employment or daycare on infant development.  相似文献   
983.
A model was presented describing the reciprocal influence of disruptions in parent discipline practices on irritable exchanges between the target child and other family members. Disrupted parent discipline and irritable microsocial exchanges within the family were hypothesized to provide a basic training for aggression that generalizes to other settings such that the child is identified by peers, teachers, and parents as physically aggressive. Physical fighting was thought to lead to rejection by the normal peer group, which was hypothesized to feed back to further exacerbate fighting. Multilevel assessment including interview, questionnaires, laboratory studies, and home observations were carried out with the families of 91 preadolescent and adolescent boys. Nine indicators from the assessment battery were used to define the constructs Inept Parental Discipline, Negative Microsocial Exchanges, Physical Fighting, and Poor Peer Relations. Structural equations (LISREL VI) were used to describe the relations among the constructs. The t values for the path coefficients were significant. A chi-square analysis showed an acceptable fit between the model and the empirical findings. The findings were interpreted as being consistent with the hypothesis that under certain circumstances, family interaction may serve as basic training for aggression. In the present study, interactions with siblings in the home seemed to serve a pivotal role.  相似文献   
984.
985.
986.
This study used the interpersonal–psychological theory of suicide to explore the relationships among DSM-5 posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) symptom clusters derived from the six-factor anhedonia model and facets of acquired capability for suicide (ACS). In a sample of 373 trauma-exposed undergraduates, most PTSD symptom clusters were negatively associated with facets of ACS in bivariate correlations, but the anhedonia cluster was positively associated with ACS in regression models. Structure coefficients and commonality analysis indicated that anhedonia served as a suppressor variable for the other symptom clusters. Our findings further elucidate the complex relationship between specific PTSD symptom clusters and ACS.  相似文献   
987.
In recent years, an impressive research program has developed around non‐analytic reductions of the normative. Nevertheless, non‐analytic naturalists face a damning dilemma: either they need to give the same reductive analysis for epistemic and practical reasons, or they can give a different analyses by treating epistemic and practical reasons as a species of the larger genus, reasonhood. Since, for example, a desire‐based account of epistemic reasons is implausible, the reductionist must opt for the latter. Yet, if the desire‐based account of practical reasons is merely a species of the larger genus, then, due to a violation of irreflexivity, the reduction fails.  相似文献   
988.
989.
990.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号