全文获取类型
收费全文 | 5189篇 |
免费 | 75篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
5265篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 49篇 |
2021年 | 66篇 |
2020年 | 103篇 |
2019年 | 104篇 |
2018年 | 183篇 |
2017年 | 156篇 |
2016年 | 165篇 |
2015年 | 93篇 |
2014年 | 106篇 |
2013年 | 459篇 |
2012年 | 177篇 |
2011年 | 148篇 |
2010年 | 111篇 |
2009年 | 136篇 |
2008年 | 136篇 |
2007年 | 142篇 |
2006年 | 115篇 |
2005年 | 110篇 |
2004年 | 102篇 |
2003年 | 82篇 |
2002年 | 82篇 |
2001年 | 137篇 |
2000年 | 102篇 |
1999年 | 88篇 |
1997年 | 48篇 |
1996年 | 47篇 |
1993年 | 53篇 |
1992年 | 72篇 |
1991年 | 77篇 |
1990年 | 65篇 |
1989年 | 60篇 |
1988年 | 73篇 |
1987年 | 64篇 |
1986年 | 52篇 |
1985年 | 64篇 |
1984年 | 63篇 |
1983年 | 57篇 |
1982年 | 58篇 |
1981年 | 46篇 |
1979年 | 62篇 |
1978年 | 63篇 |
1977年 | 57篇 |
1976年 | 48篇 |
1975年 | 69篇 |
1974年 | 63篇 |
1973年 | 56篇 |
1972年 | 52篇 |
1971年 | 53篇 |
1968年 | 50篇 |
1967年 | 62篇 |
排序方式: 共有5265条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
Clement Andrew O’Donnell Ryan E. Brockmole James R. 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2019,26(4):1266-1272
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review - A growing number of studies suggest that semantic knowledge can influence the control of gaze in scenes. For example, observers are more likely to look toward... 相似文献
962.
Jonathan?SurovellEmail authorView authors OrcID profile 《Ethical Theory and Moral Practice》2017,20(2):257-277
Many theories of moral status that are intended to ground pro-choice views on abortion tie full moral status to advanced cognitive capabilities. Extant accounts of this kind are inconsistent with the intuition that the profoundly cognitively disabled have full moral status. This paper improves upon these extant accounts by combining an anti-luck condition with Steinbock’s stratification of moral status into two levels. On the resulting view, a being has full moral status if and only if (1) she has moral status and (2) (a) has had advanced cognitive capacities, (b) has the potential to develop such capacities, or (c) would have had such capacities were it not for luck. I argue that modal accounts of luck provide a non-speciesist basis for attributing the lack of advanced cognitive capacities in humans to luck without doing the same for non-human animals. 相似文献
963.
Can Kabadayi Anastasia Krasheninnikova Laurie O’Neill Joost van de Weijer Mathias Osvath Auguste M. P. von Bayern 《Animal cognition》2017,20(6):1137-1146
The ability to inhibit unproductive motor responses triggered by salient stimuli is a fundamental inhibitory skill. Such motor self-regulation is thought to underlie more complex cognitive mechanisms, like self-control. Recently, a large-scale study, comparing 36 species, found that absolute brain size best predicted competence in motor inhibition, with great apes as the best performers. This was challenged when three Corvus species (corvids) were found to parallel great apes despite having much smaller absolute brain sizes. However, new analyses suggest that it is the number of pallial neurons, and not absolute brain size per se, that correlates with levels of motor inhibition. Both studies used the cylinder task, a detour-reaching test where food is presented behind a transparent barrier. We tested four species from the order Psittaciformes (parrots) on this task. Like corvids, many parrots have relatively large brains, high numbers of pallial neurons, and solve challenging cognitive tasks. Nonetheless, parrots performed markedly worse than the Corvus species in the cylinder task and exhibited strong learning effects in performance and response times. Our results suggest either that parrots are poor at controlling their motor impulses, and hence that pallial neuronal numbers do not always correlate with such skills, or that the widely used cylinder task may not be a good measure of motor inhibition. 相似文献
964.
Bulgheroni Maria Camperio-Ciani Andrea Straulino Elisa Sartori Luisa D’Amico Enrico Castiello Umberto 《Animal cognition》2017,20(2):359-366
Animal Cognition - When a monkey selects a piece of food lying on the ground from among other viable objects in the near vicinity, only the desired item governs the particular pattern and direction... 相似文献
965.
Ryan Francis O. Cayubit Kevin M. Rebolledo Romulo Gabriel A. Kintanar Angelissa G. Pastores Alen Josef A. Santiago Paula Bianca V. Valles 《Psychological studies》2017,62(4):386-394
In computer security parlance, a hacker is an individual who actively seeks and takes advantage of flaws and weaknesses of a computer network or system. In recent years, one has seen the rise of hacking, often as a form of protest and retaliation for reasons ranging from political to social, but the question remains as to what really motivates an individual to hack a computer network? Using the Q-method, the study aimed to develop a classification of the motivation of computer hackers. This was done with the use of 43 participants who were subjected to preliminary interviews and the sorting method. The statements from the interviews were categorized that resulted in seven conceptual themes, namely social-positive, social-negative, intellectual gain, self-satisfaction, economic rewards, technological-positive and technological-negative. From these themes, three distinct profiles emerged and were used to describe the motivation of hackers. 相似文献
966.
The current study explored the persistence of event model organizations and how this influences the experience of interference during retrieval. People in this study memorized lists of sentences about objects in locations, such as “The potted palm is in the hotel.” Previous work has shown that such information can either be stored in separate event models, thereby producing retrieval interference, or integrated into common event models, thereby eliminating retrieval interference. Unlike prior studies, the current work explored the impact of forgetting up to 2 weeks later on this pattern of performance. We explored three possible outcomes across the various retention intervals. First, consistent with research showing that longer delays reduce proactive and retroactive interference, any retrieval interference effects of competing event models could be reduced over time. Second, the binding of information into events models may weaken over time, causing interference effects to emerge when they had previously been absent. Third, and finally, the organization of information into event models could remain stable over long periods of time. The results reported here are most consistent with the last outcome. While there were some minor variations across the various retention intervals, the basic pattern of event model organization remained preserved over the two-week retention period. 相似文献
967.
968.
969.
Monin (2003) showed that the attractiveness of a face increases its perceived familiarity regardless of prior exposure, and suggested that this beautiful-is-familiar effect was due to the misattribution to familiarity of the positive affect (or “warm glow”) elicited by attractive faces. This research tests the alternative interpretation that an evaluative match between a positive stimulus (an attractive face) and a positive response (“familiar”) accounts for this effect in the absence of any misattribution. In a face recognition task, participants were led to signal their sense of familiarity with previously seen and unseen faces by selecting either a pleasant (affectively congruent) or unpleasant (affectively incongruent) image. Consistent with the warm glow heuristic, higher false alarm rates were obtained for more attractive distracters, and this effect survived (and was, if anything, stronger) when an affectively incongruent response format was used. These findings are discussed in the context of current face memory and perceptual fluency models. 相似文献
970.
Scott O. Lilienfeld 《Applied and Preventive Psychology》2004,11(1):47-51
As Meehl (1978) observed, one major reason for the painfully slow progress of soft psychology is our field’s heavy reliance on directional predictions, which leads to feeble tests of theories. In the present commentary, I outline six ways of taking theoretical risks in a world of directional predictions: (1) constructive replication of findings, (2) generating multiple maximally independent predictions, (3) adherence to the total evidence rule, (4) discriminant validation of measures, (5) incremental validation of measures, and (6) according attention to higher-order personality dimensions. 相似文献