首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   890篇
  免费   43篇
  2020年   19篇
  2019年   18篇
  2018年   14篇
  2017年   23篇
  2016年   13篇
  2015年   17篇
  2014年   18篇
  2013年   111篇
  2012年   28篇
  2011年   35篇
  2010年   20篇
  2009年   23篇
  2008年   23篇
  2007年   31篇
  2006年   21篇
  2005年   26篇
  2004年   20篇
  2003年   30篇
  2002年   20篇
  2001年   18篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   9篇
  1998年   24篇
  1997年   9篇
  1996年   17篇
  1995年   6篇
  1994年   9篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   10篇
  1991年   7篇
  1990年   8篇
  1988年   8篇
  1987年   11篇
  1986年   10篇
  1985年   14篇
  1984年   6篇
  1983年   6篇
  1982年   10篇
  1981年   17篇
  1980年   12篇
  1979年   10篇
  1978年   14篇
  1977年   12篇
  1976年   17篇
  1974年   12篇
  1973年   12篇
  1972年   6篇
  1971年   15篇
  1970年   6篇
  1966年   10篇
排序方式: 共有933条查询结果,搜索用时 922 毫秒
31.
The present study evaluated a participative management approach for increasing the frequency of interactions between institutional staff and severely/profoundly retarded residents. The participative management approach involved teaching staff how to use self-monitoring, standard setting, self-evaluation, and self-reinforcement procedures. These procedures were then used by staff with minimal involvement of supervisory personnel. Although supervisors provided feedback and praise to staff for using these self-management behaviors, feedback and praise were never dispensed contingent on staff interactions with residents. Results indicated that during the participative management program there was an increase in staff interactions that were contingent on appropriate resident behavior. The increase in this type of staff interaction was accompanied by an increase in appropriate resident behavior. Follow-up data on both staff and resident behaviors, although showing moderating trends, suggested generally good maintenance of the initial behavior changes. Acceptability data suggested that staff were quite receptive to the program. The advantages of participative management procedures for improving staff performance in residential settings are discussed.  相似文献   
32.
Potential members of the International Fluency Association (IFA) were polled regarding their fluency interests and perceived priorities for the organization during the year it was founded (in 1990). Results from 201 respondents indicated that a need for the IFA existed among researchers, clinicians, and representatives of self-help organizations for people who stutter. Improving the quality of treatment for those who stutter was considered the highest priority by all three groups. Researchers were of the opinion that the Journal of Fluency Disorders should be of higher quality than it had been in the past, and clinicians were most interested in reviews of new and relevant materials.  相似文献   
33.
34.
In three experiments, subjects imposed sentence in a criminal case before and after reading the harsh sentence imposed by the trial judge. They then read and imposed sentence in another case. In one experimental condition the subjects anticipated learning the judge's sentence in Case 2; in the other condition they were told not to anticipate learning the judge's sentence. Control subjects made their judgments without ever seeing the judge's sentence. Direct influence was the change in sentences within cases; indirect influence was the difference between initial sentences in Cases 1 and 2. Though direct influence was minimal and not significant, indirect influence was substantial, and was greater in the anticipation than no-anticipation than control conditions, indicating that both modification of the judgmental process and anticipatory conformity were operating. Indirect influence may then reflect a change of either type, depending on a number of methodological and other factors.  相似文献   
35.
In a two-stimulus matching paradigm requiring “same” and “different” responses to simple geometric dimensions, the successive stimulus presentation mode with several retention intervals was compared to a simultaneous stimulus presentation mode. For the simultaneous condition, the data suggested a wholistic template matching process, but in none of the successive conditions was template matching indicated. The retention interval of the successive condition allows the subject to analyze out the target dimension from the first two-dimensional stimulus. The ability to anticipate the value of the target dimension in the second stimulus could also influence the representation of the first stimulus. The findings shed some light upon the perceptual interactions between dimensions that constitute the stimulus. In particular, an outside-to-inside order of iconic scanning appeared to be supported by the data.  相似文献   
36.
A group of young children (mean age: 2.5 yr) were instructed to follow different requests by a teacher in a day-care setting. Experiment I verified that mean group instruction following was low (10%) despite the opportunity for “observational learning”, i.e., the group of 12 children could watch a nonreinforced adult comply with the teacher's request. In Experiment II, when positive consequences were provided contingent on the adult model's behavior, mean group instruction following was relatively unaffected (14%). When direct reinforcement was given to four peer models, each for several sessions, the individual performances of three of the four peer models was elevated (from 50% to 80%); however, the mean performance of the remaining nonreinforced children (N = 7) was only moderately affected (21%). When reinforcement contingencies were again changed, so that each group member was provided direct, but intermittent reinforcement, mean group performance increased substantially to levels of over 70%. Once instruction following was high, presentation of reinforcement only to one peer model sufficed to maintain performance whereas earlier, this same vicarious reinforcement procedure had failed to establish group compliance. The maintenance of instruction-following behavior when reinforcement was applied solely to one child was interpreted mainly in terms of a high resistance to extinction following a history of intermittent reinforcement rather than a “vicarious”- or “self”-reinforcement mechanism. Finally, removal and re-introduction of group intermittent reinforcement, respectively, lowered performance (to levels of 40%) and elevated (to levels of 65%) the group's performance.  相似文献   
37.
38.
39.
A software system for the administration of six different strategies of adaptive ability tests in a variety of formats was developed both on a large-scale time-sharing system and on a real-time minicomputer system. Factors influencing both hardware selection and software design are discussed.  相似文献   
40.
Corrado  Michael Louis 《Synthese》2021,198(3):1967-1982
Synthese - In this paper I propose a form of free will fallibilism. Unlike the free will realist who is fully persuaded that we have sufficient evidence of freedom to justify holding individuals...  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号