全文获取类型
收费全文 | 34832篇 |
免费 | 652篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
35486篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 335篇 |
2019年 | 458篇 |
2018年 | 577篇 |
2017年 | 612篇 |
2016年 | 611篇 |
2015年 | 444篇 |
2014年 | 556篇 |
2013年 | 2503篇 |
2012年 | 1015篇 |
2011年 | 1087篇 |
2010年 | 637篇 |
2009年 | 637篇 |
2008年 | 936篇 |
2007年 | 924篇 |
2006年 | 863篇 |
2005年 | 780篇 |
2004年 | 753篇 |
2003年 | 701篇 |
2002年 | 726篇 |
2001年 | 1041篇 |
2000年 | 953篇 |
1999年 | 732篇 |
1998年 | 407篇 |
1996年 | 334篇 |
1992年 | 564篇 |
1991年 | 555篇 |
1990年 | 540篇 |
1989年 | 549篇 |
1988年 | 543篇 |
1987年 | 514篇 |
1986年 | 540篇 |
1985年 | 589篇 |
1984年 | 464篇 |
1983年 | 442篇 |
1982年 | 372篇 |
1981年 | 362篇 |
1979年 | 535篇 |
1978年 | 392篇 |
1977年 | 325篇 |
1976年 | 380篇 |
1975年 | 437篇 |
1974年 | 502篇 |
1973年 | 523篇 |
1972年 | 419篇 |
1971年 | 399篇 |
1970年 | 386篇 |
1969年 | 412篇 |
1968年 | 493篇 |
1967年 | 427篇 |
1966年 | 433篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
921.
M. Todd Allen Catherine E. Myers Mark A. Gluck 《Integrative psychological & behavioral science》2001,36(1):36-61
Classical conditioning has been explained by two main types of theories that postulate different learning mechanisms. Rescorla
and Wagner (1972) put forth a theory in which conditioning is based on the ability of the US to drive learning through error
correction. Alternatively, Mackintosh (1973) put forth a theory in which the ability of the CS to be associated with the unconditioned
stimulus is modulated. We have proposed a reconciliation of these two mechanisms as working in parallel within different neural
systems: a cerebellar system for US modulation and a hippocampal system for CS modulation. We developed a computational model
of cerebellar function in eyeblink conditioning based on the error correction mechanism of the Rescorla-Wagner rule in which
learningrelated activity from the cerebellum inhibits the inferior olive, which is the US input pathway to the cerebellum
(Gluck et al., 1994). We developed a computational model of the hippocampal region that forms altered representations of conditioned
stimuli based on their behavioral outcomes (Gluck & Myers, 1993; Myers et al., 1995). Overall, computational modeling and
empirical findings support the idea that, at least in the case of eyeblink conditioning, there may be two different neural
systems: the cerebellum which mediates US-based error correction and hippocampus which alters representations of CSs. 相似文献
922.
The Internet has been exploited successfully in the past as a medium for behavioral research. This paper presents a series of studies designed to assess Internet-based measures of drivers’ risk-taking behavior. First, we compared responses from an Internet sample with a traditional pencil-and-paper sample using established questionnaire measures of risk taking. No significant differences were found. Second, we assessed the validity of new Internet-based instruments, involving photographs and photographic animations, that measured speed, gap acceptance, and passing. Responses were found to reflect known demographic patterns of actual behavior to some degree. Also, a roadside survey of speeds was carried out at the locations depicted in the photographic measure of speeding and, with certain exceptions, differences between the two appeared to be constant. Third, a between-subject experimental manipulation involving the photographic animation measure of gap acceptance was used to demonstrate one application of these techniques. 相似文献
923.
High levels of false recognition are observed after people study lists of semantic associates that all converge on a nonpresented lure word. In previous experiments, we have found that orienting participants to encode distinctive information about study list items by presenting them as pictures as opposed to words produces marked reductions in false recognition. We have suggested that these reductions reflect the operation of a distinctiveness heuristic: Participants demand access to detailed pictorial information in order to support a positive recognition decision. The present experiments provide additional evidence on this point and allow us to distinguish between the distinctiveness heuristic account and an alternative account based on the impoverished encoding of relational information that occurs when one is studying pictures. In Experiment 1, even when only half of the items in a study list were presented as pictures, a general suppression of false recognition was observed that could be attributable to impoverished encoding of relational information. Experiment 2 provided a critical test of the distinctiveness heuristic account: We manipulated test instructions and found that differences in false recognition rates between picture and word encoding were attenuated in a retrieval condition that did not encourage reliance on a distinctiveness heuristic. 相似文献
924.
A curriculum of about 22 hr distributed over 3 months on some core political notions (law, the main Italian political offices, the state) was implemented in an Italian 3rd-grade class of 23 children. Another class of 20 children formed the comparison group. The workings of the executive system were exemplified by explaining the running of the school (which in Italy is state-run). Children's learning and retention of these notions were tested twice, 1 month and 10 months after the end of the curriculum. At pre-test, before implementation of the curriculum, the majority of the children did not have a concept of the political domain: They did not know of parliament and government, hardly distinguished between the law and other types of rules, and did not know that teachers, policemen, and judges are public servants. At post- and delayed post-test, most of the children in the experimental group had mastered the concepts, performing significantly better than the comparison group. Thus, the development of political understanding appears to be due to the acquisition of relevant domain-specific knowledge rather than the emergence of across-the-board logic ability. 相似文献
925.
The third edition of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale manual reports four-factor solutions for the WAIS-III, and subsequent research has validated four-factor solutions for a variety of samples. These four factors consistently correspond to the four Factor Indexes that are yielded by the WAIS-III. However, the WAIS-III still provides Verbal and Performance IQs, in addition to the Indexes, making it desirable to examine two-factor solutions as well. In addition, because the Wechsler literature includes much interpretation of three-factor solutions, these solutions were likewise examined. Principal factor analysis followed by Varimax and Oblimin rotations of two and three factors were performed on data for the total WAIS-III sample ages 16 to 89 years (N=2,450). The two-factor solutions were viewed as a construct validation of Wechsler's two separate IQs, although the Working Memory subtests tended to load higher on the Performance scale than on their intended scale (Verbal); three-factor solutions were interpreted within the context of Horn's expanded fluid-crystallized theory and research on working memory. Both the two- and three-factor Varimax-rotated solutions were related to similar factor analyses conducted previously for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-III. Coefficients of congruence between like-named factors consistently exceeded .90, and usually .98, across different Wechsler batteries. 相似文献
926.
What factors contribute to hindsight bias, the phenomenon whereby the known outcome of an event appears obvious only after
the fact? The Causal Model Theory (CMT) of hindsight bias (Nestler et al. in Soc Psychol 39:182–188, 2008a; in J Expl Psychol: Learn Mem Cog 34:1043–1054, 2008b; Pezzo in Mem 11:421–441, 2003; Wasserman et al. in Pers Soc Psychol Bull 17:30–35, 1991) posits that hindsight bias can occur when people have the opportunity to identify potential causal antecedents and evaluate
whether they could have led to the outcome. Two experiments incorporating highly controlled minimalist scenarios supported
the CMT. As predicted by the CMT, hindsight bias occurred when the causal factor explained the actual outcome better than
the alternative outcome, and reverse hindsight bias occurred when the causal factor explained the alternative outcome better
than the actual outcome. Moreover, we found new evidence that outcome knowledge alone was insufficient to elicit hindsight
bias in the absence of a potential causal antecedent. Implications for future directions in hindsight bias research are discussed. 相似文献
927.
Polder-Verkiel SE 《Science and engineering ethics》2012,18(1):117-141
In 2008 a young man committed suicide while his webcam was running. 1,500 people apparently watched as the young man lay dying:
when people finally made an effort to call the police, it was too late. This closely resembles the case of Kitty Genovese
in 1964, where 39 neighbours supposedly watched an attacker assault and did not call until it was too late. This paper examines
the role of internet mediation in cases where people may or may not have been good Samaritans and what their responsibilities
were. The method is an intuitive one: intuitions on the various potentially morally relevant differences when it comes to
responsibility between offline and online situations are examined. The number of onlookers, their physical nearness and their
anonymity have no moral relevance when it comes to holding them responsible. Their perceived reality of the situation and
ability to act do have an effect on whether we can hold people responsible, but this doesn’t seem to be unique to internet
mediation. However the way in which those factors are intrinsically connected to internet mediation does seem to have a diminishing
effect on responsibility in online situations. 相似文献
928.
In this paper we shed new light on the Argument from Disagreement by putting it to test in a computer simulation. According
to this argument widespread and persistent disagreement on ethical issues indicates that our moral opinions are not influenced
by any moral facts, either because no such facts exist or because they are epistemically inaccessible or inefficacious for
some other reason. Our simulation shows that if our moral opinions were influenced at least a little bit by moral facts, we
would quickly have reached consensus, even if our moral opinions were affected by factors such as false authorities, external
political shifts, and random processes. Therefore, since no such consensus has been reached, the simulation gives us increased
reason to take seriously the Argument from Disagreement. Our conclusion is however not conclusive; the simulation also indicates
what assumptions one has to make in order to reject the Argument from Disagreement. The simulation algorithm we use builds
on the work of Hegselmann and Krause (J Artif Soc Social Simul 5(3); 2002, J Artif Soc Social Simul 9(3), 2006). 相似文献
929.
Chengwu Yang Anbesaw W. Selassie Rickey E. Carter Barbara C. Tilley 《Applied research in quality of life》2012,7(1):63-81
This study demonstrated reliability and factor structure of the Medical Outcomes Study Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36) among
older Americans with Traumatic brain injury (TBI), and evaluated effects of injury severity and race on SF-36's items and
latent domains. A representative sample of 654 older, racially diverse patients with TBI was selected from the South Carolina
Traumatic Brain Injury Follow-up Registry. Reliability and factor structure of SF-36 were evaluated using Cronbach’s alpha
and confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). Multiple-indicator multiple-causes (MIMIC) models were used to study effects of injury
severity and race on items (differential item functioning, DIF) and latent domains (population heterogeneity) of SF-36. SF-36
was reliable and its current eightfactor structure was confirmed. While TBI severity did not impact latent domain scores of
SF-36, race did. Blacks had higher vitality and lower role-emotional scores than whites. The measurement model was invariant
to injury severity and race (free of DIF), and DIF did not contribute to the differences of vitality and role-emotional between
black and white older TBI patients. SF-36 was valid to measure quality of life (OoL) after TBI in racially diverse elderly
population. Blacks tend to assert to strong coping behaviors in the presence of physical stress while admitting low performance
due to emotional stress. In QoL research where the primary outcomes are usually composite scores from instruments, MIMIC models
have advantages over conventional multivariable regression models in testing the validity of the instruments and assessing
covariate effects on latent traits of instruments while controlling for DIF effects. 相似文献
930.