全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1040篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
1049篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 20篇 |
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 9篇 |
2021年 | 17篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 33篇 |
2018年 | 45篇 |
2017年 | 48篇 |
2016年 | 67篇 |
2015年 | 47篇 |
2014年 | 39篇 |
2013年 | 133篇 |
2012年 | 59篇 |
2011年 | 61篇 |
2010年 | 42篇 |
2009年 | 46篇 |
2008年 | 39篇 |
2007年 | 46篇 |
2006年 | 34篇 |
2005年 | 29篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 27篇 |
2002年 | 35篇 |
2001年 | 11篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 8篇 |
1998年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 4篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 2篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1985年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1970年 | 1篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1967年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
1960年 | 1篇 |
1956年 | 1篇 |
1938年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有1049条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
241.
Three studies support the vicarious dissonance hypothesis that individuals change their attitudes when witnessing members of important groups engage in inconsistent behavior. Study 1, in which participants observed an actor in an induced-compliance paradigm, documented that students who identified with their college supported an issue more after hearing an ingroup member make a counterattitudinal speech in favor of that issue. In Study 2, vicarious dissonance occurred even when participants did not hear a speech, and attitude change was highest when the speaker was known to disagree with the issue. Study 3 showed that speaker choice and aversive consequences moderated vicarious dissonance, and demonstrated that vicarious discomfort--the discomfort observers imagine feeling if in an actor's place--was attenuated after participants expressed their revised attitudes. 相似文献
242.
The sequence of neurophysiological processes elicited in the auditory system by a sound is analyzed in search of the stage at which the processes carrying sensory information cross the borderline beyond which they directly underlie sound perception. Neurophysiological data suggest that this transition occurs when the sensory input is mapped onto the physiological basis of sensory memory in the auditory cortex. At this point, the sensory information carried by the stimulus-elicited process corresponds, for the first time, to that contained by the actual sound percept. Before this stage, the sensory stimulus code is fragmentary, lacks the time dimension, cannot enter conscious perception, and is not accessible to top-down processes (voluntary mental operations). On these grounds, 2 distinct stages of auditory sensory processing, prerepresentational and representational, can be distinguished. 相似文献
243.
Coping with neck and low back pain in the general population. 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
This study had 2 objectives: (a) to assess the psychometric properties of the Pain Management Inventory (PMI; G. K. Brown & P. M. Nicassio, 1987) with individuals in the general population with neck or low back pain, and (b) to assess the relationship between pain severity and coping. Data were taken from a mailed survey of a random sample of adults residing in Saskatchewan, Canada. Results validated the 2-factor structure of the PMI and provided evidence for the internal consistency of the coping subscales. Regression analyses revealed that passive coping was associated with being married, greater pain severity, depression, and poor health. Active coping was associated with female gender, higher education, less depression, good health, and frequent exercise. This study provides psychometric data to support the use of the PMI and information about factors associated with use of active and passive coping strategies in pain sufferers. 相似文献
244.
245.
The medical concept of prognosis is analysed into its basic constituents: patient data, medical intervention, outcome, utilities and probabilities; and sources of utility and probability values are discussed. Prognosis cannot be divorced from contemplated medical action, nor from action to be taken by the patient in response to prognostication. Regrettably, the usual decision-theoretic approach ignores this latter aspect. Elicitation of utilities, decision contemplation and prognostic counselling interweave, diagnostics playing a subsidiary role in decision-oriented clinical practice. At times the doctor has grounds for withholding information. As this is known to the patient, prognostic counselling becomes a conflict-prone and rationality-thwarting activity. The meaning of standard phrases such as “prognosis of a disease”, “the prognosis of this patient”, “the prognosis is unknown”, is examined. 相似文献
246.
Røysamb E Tambs K Reichborn-Kjennerud T Neale MC Harris JR 《Journal of personality and social psychology》2003,85(6):1136-1146
The aim was to identify genetic and environmental influences on the covariances between subjective well-being (SWB), perceived health, and somatic illness. Analyses were based on 6576 Norwegian twins aged 18-31. Heritabilities ranged from .24 to.66. SWB correlated .50 with perceived health, -.25 with musculoskeletal pain, and -.07 with allergy. Common genetic factors accounted for 45%-60% of associations. SWB and perceived health was to a high extent influenced by the same genes (r(g)=.72 and.82 for males and females, respectively). For SWB and musculoskeletal pain, r-sub(g) =-.29 and -.42 for males and females, respectively. Effects were partly sex specific. Environmental factors shared by twins did not affect the covariances. Results support a differentiated view of SWB-health relations, and imply that both genes and environment play important roles in the associations between well-being and health. 相似文献
247.
Winkler I Sussman E Tervaniemi M Horváth J Ritter W Näätänen R 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2003,3(1):57-77
The effects of auditory context on the preattentive and perceptual organization of tone sequences were investigated. Two sets
of experiments were conducted in which the pitch of contextual tones was varied, bringing about two different contextual manipulations.
Preattentive auditory organization was indexed by the mismatch negativity event-related potential, which is elicited by violations
of auditory regularities even when participants ignore the sounds (e.g., by reading a book). The perceptual effects of the
contextual manipulations on auditory grouping were assessed using target-detection and orderjudgment tasks. The close correspondence
found between the effects of auditory context on the perceptual and preattentive measures of auditory grouping suggests that
a large part of contextual processing is preattentive. 相似文献
248.
249.
Shtyrov Y Kujala T Lyytinen H Kujala J Ilmoniemi RJ Näätänen R 《Brain and cognition》2000,43(1-3):392-398
The goal of the present study was to evaluate the differences between dichotic listening and mismatch negativity as measures of speech lateralization in the human brain. For this purpose, we recorded the magnetic equivalent of the mismatch negativity, elicited by consonant-vowel syllable change, and tested the same subjects in the dichotic listening procedure. The results showed that both methods indicated left-hemisphere dominance in speech processing. However, the mismatch negativity, as compared to the right-ear advantage, suggested slightly stronger left-hemisphere dominance in speech processing. No clear correlation was found between the laterality indexes of mismatch negativity and right-ear advantage calculated from dichotic listening results. The possible explanation for this finding would be that these two measures reflect different stages of speech processing in the human brain. 相似文献
250.
István Winkler Kalevi Reinikainen Risto Näätänen 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1993,53(4):443-449
In sequences of identical auditory stimuli, infrequent deviant stimuli elicit an event-related brain potential component called mismatch negativity (MMN). MMN is presumed to reflect the existence of a memory trace of the frequent stimulus at the moment of presentation of the infrequent stimulus. This hypothesis was tested by applying the recognition-masking paradigm of cognitive psychology. In this paradigm, a masking sound presented shortly before or after a test stimulus diminishes the recognition memory of this stimulus, the more so the shorter the interval between the test and masking stimuli. This interval was varied in the present study. It was found that the MMN amplitude strongly correlated with the subject’s ability to discriminate between frequent and infrequent stimuli. This result strongly suggests that MMN providesa measure for a trace of sensory memory, and further, that with MMN, this memory can be studied without performance-related distortions. 相似文献