全文获取类型
收费全文 | 439篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
448篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 1篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 7篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 10篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 16篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 10篇 |
2014年 | 9篇 |
2013年 | 37篇 |
2012年 | 19篇 |
2011年 | 9篇 |
2010年 | 7篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 29篇 |
2007年 | 17篇 |
2006年 | 20篇 |
2005年 | 21篇 |
2004年 | 23篇 |
2003年 | 23篇 |
2002年 | 18篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 12篇 |
1998年 | 10篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 10篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 9篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 1篇 |
1979年 | 1篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有448条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
31.
For both adults and children, acoustic context plays an important role in speech perception. For adults, both speech and nonspeech acoustic contexts influence perception of subsequent speech items, consistent with the argument that effects of context are due to domain-general auditory processes. However, prior research examining the effects of context on children’s speech perception have focused on speech contexts; nonspeech contexts have not been explored previously. To better understand the developmental progression of children’s use of contexts in speech perception and the mechanisms underlying that development, we created a novel experimental paradigm testing 5-year-old children’s speech perception in several acoustic contexts. The results demonstrated that nonspeech context influences children’s speech perception, consistent with claims that context effects arise from general auditory system properties rather than speech-specific mechanisms. This supports theoretical accounts of language development suggesting that domain-general processes play a role across the lifespan. 相似文献
32.
33.
Lori L. Fazzino 《Journal of Contemporary Religion》2014,29(2):249-266
The motivating factors and structural processes of religious exits have been important inquiries in the sociology of religion and are increasingly important to the field of non-religious studies. Based on qualitative research with 20 former post-Boomer Evangelical Christians, this article takes a phenomenological–hermeneutical approach to understanding religious exiting by examining narratives of deconversion. The narrative data presented in this article depict Evangelical exits from a deconversion perspective where exiters emphasize breaking away from the constraints of hegemonic Christianity rather than turning to secularity. The findings suggest that framing the intentional rejection of faith as ‘deconversion’ transforms exit narratives into a necessary cultural repertoire that encourages individuals to challenge religious domination and makes finding acceptance and validation easier in an established community of non-believers. By emphasizing the negative impact of religion, even after non-religious worldviews have been adopted, deconverts come to know who they currently are in the light of who they once were. 相似文献
34.
Gabrielle B Britton Lori B Astheimer 《Integrative Physiological and Behavioral Science》2004,39(4):295-306
In classical eyeblink conditioning, non-specific emotional responses to the aversive shock unconditioned stimulus (US), which are presumed to coincide with the development of fear, occur early in conditioning and precede the emergence of eyeblink responses. This two-process learning model was examined by concurrently measuring fear and eyeblink conditioning in the freely moving rat. Freezing served as an index of fear in animals and was measured during the inter-trial intervals in the training context and during a tone conditioned stimulus (CS) presented in a novel context. Animals that received CS-US pairings exhibited elevated levels of fear to the context and CS early in training that decreased over sessions, while eyeblink conditioned responses (CRs) developed gradually during acquisition and decreased during extinction. Random CS-US presentations produced a similar pattern of fear responses to the context and CS as paired presentations despite low eyeblink CR percentages, indicating that fear responding was decreased independent of high levels of learned eyeblink responding. The results of paired training were consistent with two-process models of conditioning that postulate that early emotional responding facilitates subsequent motor learning, but measures from random control animals demonstrate that partial CS-US contingencies produce decrements in fear despite low levels of eyeblink CRs. These findings suggest a relationship between CS-US contingency and fear levels during eyeblink conditioning, and may serve to clarify further the role that fear conditioning plays in this simple paradigm. 相似文献
35.
Szalda-Petree AD Craft BB Martin LM Deditius-Island HK 《Perceptual and motor skills》2004,98(1):141-146
Previous research on self-control using macaques (Macaca fascicularis) showed these animals have a strong bias for a delayed, larger reinforcer (Self-control) over an immediate, smaller reinforcer (Impulsive). Typical studies of self-control have used a discrete trials methodology with a secondary discriminative stimulus during the delay periods. This results in a greater exposure to the stimulus representing the self-controlled option and may account for some of the early exclusive preference for self-control observed. The present experiment examined self-control bias in three rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) while controlling for differential durations of stimulus exposure. Subjects were presented stimuli via a computer monitor and made choices by touching the stimulus at which point both stimuli were removed for the delay periods. All three subjects displayed a nearly exclusive bias for the delayed, larger reinforcer self-control). These results are consistent with previous studies, despite the variations in methodology and species. 相似文献
36.
Erica L. Hauck Lori Anderson Snyder Luz-Eugenia Cox-Fuenzalida 《Current Psychology》2008,27(2):112-125
The current study investigated the effect of workload variability on performance and stress, as well as the role of social
support as a potential intervention. Based on previous research it was predicted that a shift in workload from either high
to low or low to high would lower performance and increase stress, while the provision of social support would result in higher
performance and lower stress. Results revealed significant decrements in performance following workload shifts, though no
increase in stress was perceived. Performance in both workload conditions significantly, but inconsistently, increased following
the provision of social support and stress significantly decreased following the shift from high to low workload. Implications
and future research steps are discussed further. 相似文献
37.
Laura Perrone Jessica L. Borelli Patricia Smiley Hannah F. Rasmussen Lori M. Hilt 《Journal of child and family studies》2016,25(11):3387-3402
Both parental conditional regard for academics and depressogenic attributions are related to detrimental psychological outcomes for children. Here we examine associations among parental conditional negative regard, child depressogenic attributions, child depressive symptoms, and emotion reactivity in children between the ages of 8 and 12, as well as whether children’s self-reported and behavioral attributions for negative events mediate associations between parental conditional negative regard for academics with children’s depressive symptoms and emotion reactivity. In Study 1 (N?=?108, M age ?=?9.73, 50 male), children’s self-reported attributions for hypothetical events mediated the link between parental conditional negative regard and child depressive symptoms. In Study 2 (N?=?104, M age ?=?10.28, 54 male), children attempted an impossible puzzle task while their skin conductance level was monitored, after which they completed an interview that was coded for spontaneous attributions for failure. Children’s spontaneous attributions mediated the link between parental conditional negative regard and child emotion reactivity, but not depressive symptoms. Findings suggest that children’s attributions may be a mechanism through which parental conditional negative regard is related to children’s depressive symptoms and emotion reactivity during a performance challenge. These results have implications for developmental models of depression risk and potential areas for clinical interventions with both children and their parents. 相似文献
38.
Leslie A. Rescorla Thomas M. Achenbach Masha Y. Ivanova Lori V. Turner Hervör Árnadóttir Alma Au J. C. Caldas Yi-Chuen Chen Jeroen Decoster Johnny Fontaine Yasuko Funabiki Halldór S. Guðmundsson Patrick Leung Jianghong Liu Jelena Srdanović Maraš Jasminka Marković Kyung Ja Oh Marina M. da Rocha Virginia C. Samaniego Edwiges Silvares Roma Simulioniene Elvisa Sokoli Natalia Vazquez Ewa Zasepa 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》2016,38(3):381-397
To advance international mental health assessment, instruments that have been internationally validated are needed. To this end, we analyzed ratings from 14 societies on the Adult Behavior Checklist (ABCL), a collateral-report form parallel to the Adult Self-Report (ASR; Achenbach and Rescorla 2003) for ages 18 to 59. Both the ABCL and the ASR assess problems, personal strengths, and adaptive functioning. For a sample of 8322 see note below collaterals, we found strong consistency across societies regarding which ABCL problem items tended to obtain relatively low, medium, or high ratings. Most societal effect sizes (ESs) for problem scale scores were small to medium (< 13.9 %), but the ES for the ABCL Personal Strengths scale was 25 %. For most of the same participants (N = 8,302), we analyzed cross-informant agreement between self-reports on the ASR and collateral reports on the ABCL. Cross-informant correlations for problem scale scores averaged .47, with considerable societal variation. Problem score means were higher on the ASR than the ABCL in every society, but the size of the difference varied across societies. Mean item ratings on the ABCL and ASR were highly correlated within every society (mean r = .92), but within-dyad item rating agreement varied widely in every society (mean r = .39). In all societies, non-corroboration of self-reported deviance and of collateral-reported deviance was common. Overall findings indicated considerable similarity but also some important differences in collateral-reported problems and adaptive functioning across 14 societies. 相似文献
39.
Increasing Single‐Word Requests to Multiword Requests in Children with Autism and Related Disabilities 下载免费PDF全文
Rachel N. Yosick Lori R. Muskat Crystal N. Bowen Caitlin H. Delfs M. Alice Shillingsburg 《Behavioral Interventions》2016,31(1):28-43
Initial language training with children with autism often focuses on the production of single‐word requests (i.e., mands). As a child progresses in treatment, it is desirable to increase the mean length of utterance (MLU) of requests. The present study examined treatment outcomes of behavioral intervention designed to increase the MLU of requests in a larger sample of children with language delays (n = 30). Intervention consisted of prompts and differential reinforcement for lengthier requests, and trials were conducted flexibly throughout the treatment session. A non‐concurrent multiple baseline across participants design was utilized to establish experimental control, and MLU intervention consisted of a baseline and treatment phase for each participant. Nonoverlap of all pairs (NAP) was employed to estimate an effect size. Overall treatment effects were medium to large (average NAP = 0.89; average d = 2.36), and the majority of the sample (70%) demonstrated strong treatment effects. Results suggest that targeted behavioral intervention to increase the length of requests can be effective for children with autism and associated language delays. Copyright © 2015 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
40.
Some of the most important information we encounter changes so rapidly that our perceptual systems cannot process all of it in detail. Spatially selective attention is critical for perception when more information than can be processed in detail is presented simultaneously at distinct locations. When presented with complex, rapidly changing information, listeners may need to selectively attend to specific times rather than to locations. We present evidence that listeners can direct selective attention to time points that differ by as little as 500 msec, and that doing so improves target detection, affects baseline neural activity preceding stimulus presentation, and modulates auditory evoked potentials at a perceptually early stage. These data demonstrate that attentional modulation of early perceptual processing is temporally precise and that listeners can flexibly allocate temporally selective attention over short intervals, making it a viable mechanism for preferentially processing the most relevant segments in rapidly changing streams. 相似文献