首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   409篇
  免费   24篇
  国内免费   41篇
  474篇
  2023年   5篇
  2022年   5篇
  2020年   9篇
  2019年   7篇
  2018年   13篇
  2017年   12篇
  2016年   9篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   15篇
  2013年   33篇
  2012年   10篇
  2011年   16篇
  2010年   19篇
  2009年   15篇
  2008年   19篇
  2007年   21篇
  2006年   22篇
  2005年   22篇
  2004年   16篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   15篇
  2001年   4篇
  2000年   8篇
  1999年   10篇
  1998年   8篇
  1996年   4篇
  1994年   6篇
  1992年   13篇
  1991年   7篇
  1989年   4篇
  1988年   8篇
  1987年   5篇
  1986年   3篇
  1985年   3篇
  1984年   4篇
  1983年   6篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   5篇
  1977年   5篇
  1976年   3篇
  1975年   4篇
  1974年   5篇
  1973年   5篇
  1972年   4篇
  1971年   6篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   3篇
  1968年   6篇
  1967年   3篇
  1965年   4篇
排序方式: 共有474条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
This study investigated how group sex composition affects female and male students' perceptions of female instructors who teach in the area of counseling women. Three components of person perception were assessed: cognitive, affective, and behavioral. The cognitive domain included evaluations of the instructor and the subject matter presented; the affective domain included emotional responses to the instructor and the subject matter; and the behavioral domain included actions in response to the instructor, operationalized as the number of questions asked by students. Students with some interest in a career in the helping professions viewed one of two videotapes either with a group of same-sex peers or in a group in which they were the only male or female. One tape described a gender-neutral topic (psychology and the law) and the other described a female gender-related topic (sex bias in counseling women). The same female instructor made both presentations. Students then completed the measures. As predicted, the sex composition of the student group affected person perception, especially in the affective and behavior domains. Recommendations are made for future research.This study was supported by a small grant from the University Research Institute, University of Texas at Austin. Appreciation is expressed to Sherry Fowler, Bob Haverstick, and Kathy Thomas for help with data collection and to Carolyn Bates and Ken Wilgus for coding the open-ended responses. A paper based on this research was presented at the meetings of the American Psychological Association, August 1987.  相似文献   
102.
103.
An adaptation-test paradigm was used in two experiments examining processes underlying the perceived reversals of a rotating Necker cube. Adaptation and test cubes were either the same or different with respect to their visual fields of presentation (Experiment 1) or their sizes (Experiment 2). Results of both experiments indicated that, following subjects’ adaptation to a different cube, reversal rate of the test cube did not differ from that obtained without prior adaptation experience. In contrast, reversal rate of the test cube was elevated following adaptation to the same cube. Additional findings of Experiment 1 were that a test cube presented to the same visual field as the adaptation cube yielded a higher reversal rate than did a simultaneously presented cube in the opposite visual field. Also, the reversal rate of one cube was not influenced by the simultaneous presentation of a second cube. Results of both experiments were interpreted in terms of the fatigue and recovery of multiple, largely independent, localized neural channels. Thus, the results tie reversible-figure illusions to other visual phenomena thought to involve similar fatigue processes within localized visual channels (e.g., tilt, motion, and size aftereffects).  相似文献   
104.
Following the successive presentation of two masking gratings (M1 and M2), subjects were required to detect the presence or absence of a single vertical line (TS). When the orientations of the two masks were optimal, M1 was able to reduce the masking effect of M2 on the TS. For a vertical TS, disinhibition was maximal when the orientations of M1 and M2 were similar and was minimal when the orientational difference was greater than 15 deg. It is suggested that the spatial selectivity of the disinhibition function reflects the activity of neurons tuned to orientation, and that the disinhibition masking paradigm may be a useful psychophysical technique to measure tuning functions of other feature detectors.  相似文献   
105.
106.
Employee silence is a costly but omnipresent phenomenon in modern organisations. In this study, we focus on two forms of silence: defensive silence based on fear and acquiescent silence based on resignation. Given the power imbalance in supervisor–subordinate relationships, we hypothesise that abusive supervision is an antecedent of subordinates’ defensive silence and that a subordinate’s power distance orientation affects acquiescent silence. We investigate the interaction effects of abusive supervision and power distance orientation on these two types of silence. Perceived organisational politics may also aggravate such interactive effects. Based on data collected from 159 junior employees in China in two periods, we find that abusive supervision is associated with employee defensive silence and moderates high-power-distance employees’ tendency to engage in acquiescent silence. When perceiving high politics in the organisation, high-power-distance employees are more sensitive to abusive supervision and engage in more defensive silence. A highly political organisational context also accentuates abusive supervision’s moderating effect on the relation between employees’ power distance orientation and acquiescent silence. We conclude with theoretical and practical implications for the silence literature.  相似文献   
107.
王燕  龙立荣  周浩  祖伟 《心理学报》2007,39(2):335-342
以160名中学教师为被试,采用2×2的完全随机设计,以模拟故事(scenarios)的方法呈现刺激,研究了在职称评定中分配不公正的前提下,程序公正/不公正,互动公正/不公正对教师的退缩行为(消极怠工、拒绝帮助、离职、对校长的消极态度)的影响。结果表明,在分配不公正条件下:(1)程序公正、互动公正均影响教师的消极怠工程度;(2)程序公正、互动公正均影响教师的拒绝帮助行为程度,而且交互作用显著;(3)互动公正影响教师对上司的消极态度,程序公正无显著影响;(4)程序公正和互动公正对离职意愿影响均不显著  相似文献   
108.
G M Long  P M Garvey 《Perception》1988,17(6):745-751
The effects of target borders on the ability of observers to resolve moving targets (Landolt Cs) under a range of conditions were examined. Contrary to reported findings with stationary targets, it was predicted that the presence of borders would improve acuity for slow-moving targets because (i) overall stimulus energy is kept relatively constant as target detail varies, and (ii) a low-spatial-frequency component is held constant as target detail varies. In an experiment in which a two-sided border (above and below the target) was used, the predicted beneficial effect of the border at slow speeds was obtained. The results are discussed in terms of practical implications for the assessment of dynamic visual acuity as well as the potential neural mechanisms underlying performance.  相似文献   
109.
110.
消费伦理的变迁与当代家庭消费伦理之建构   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
消费伦理是指人们在消费水平、消费方式等问题上产生的道德观念、道德规范以及对社会消费行为的价值判断和道德评价。儒家消费伦理在历史上对于调节社会和家庭消费起过非常重要的作用,但在新中国成立以后,尤其是在市场经济改革日益深化的时代背景下,其消费伦理发生了深刻的变化。建构当代家庭消费伦理,既要弘扬和汲取传统消费伦理的合理内核,又要充分反映当今消费文明对家庭消费的客观要求,以合理适度消费和统筹兼顾消费为基本要义。  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号