全文获取类型
收费全文 | 521篇 |
免费 | 46篇 |
国内免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
576篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 5篇 |
2022年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 13篇 |
2017年 | 12篇 |
2016年 | 10篇 |
2015年 | 6篇 |
2014年 | 18篇 |
2013年 | 51篇 |
2012年 | 13篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 20篇 |
2009年 | 22篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 20篇 |
2006年 | 24篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 17篇 |
2003年 | 13篇 |
2002年 | 18篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 9篇 |
1999年 | 11篇 |
1998年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 6篇 |
1994年 | 7篇 |
1992年 | 13篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1989年 | 6篇 |
1988年 | 8篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1977年 | 5篇 |
1975年 | 7篇 |
1974年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1972年 | 5篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1967年 | 4篇 |
1965年 | 4篇 |
1962年 | 4篇 |
排序方式: 共有576条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
151.
A naturalistic study of spatial memory having temporary utility was conducted using 32 faculty and staff of a university, who park in non-reserved parking spaces, as subjects. The subjects, who were unaware of the study until interviewed, were asked to indicate on a map the exact location where they had parked their car on that day and on each of the three preceding days. Whereas retention accuracy decreased significantly across days, the recency effect was weak and overall retention proved to be very accurate. Of the subjects who varied their parking location, 88 per cent were able to indicate within three spaces where they parked on the day of the interview, and 58 per cent on the most remote day. Younger females made significantly larger retention errors across the four days than older females and younger and older males. Subjects' overall retention accuracy correlated with their overall confidence in their recollections (γ = .72). Daily ratings of ‘how busy on the job’, reported times left campus each day, parking location variability, particular parking lot used, and number of parking spaces in the distinct locations used were not significantly correlated with retention errors. In follow-up interviews, subjects reported preferring a behavioural, ‘park in a favourite spot’ strategy, with several types of imagery-based cognitive strategies being cited less frequently, and verbal encoding being the least often cited strategy. 相似文献
152.
In his widely reprinted paper “On the Folly of Rewarding A, While Hoping for B,” Kerr argued that vividness was one of the major reasons for distorted rewards. Using both archival and survey data, the present paper directly tests Kerr's proposal by investigating whether, how, and why highly visible behaviors are over‐rewarded and less visible, but similarly (or more) important behaviors are under‐rewarded. The National Basketball Association (NBA) was chosen as the domain of this study because scoring is particularly vivid, even though both non‐scoring and scoring performances are critical for winning games. Findings from four studies demonstrated that the scoring performance of NBA players was weighed more heavily than their non‐scoring performance. Scorers were rewarded with higher salaries and received more support in the NBA All‐Star balloting than defenders, even though they might not necessarily make more contribution than their teammates. This pattern of findings suggests that the vividness effect may lead to pronounced differences in people's judgments, especially when they face abundant real‐world information with similar validity. Copyright © 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
153.
154.
Two experiments examined how observers' ability to perceive biological motion changes with increasing age. The observers discriminated among kinetic figures, depicting walking, jogging, and skipping. The direction, duration, and temporal correspondence of the motions were manipulated. Quantitative differences occurred between the recognition performances of younger and older observers, but these differences were often modest. The older and younger observers' performances were comparable for most conditions at stimulus durations of 400 ms. The older observers also performed well above chance at shorter durations of 240 and 120 ms. Unlike their performance on other 2- or 3-dimensional motion tasks, older observers' ability to perceive biological motion is relatively well preserved. 相似文献
155.
The purpose of this study was to investigate the prevalence and characteristics of habit behaviors exhibited by individuals with mental retardation living in the community. Two hundred and fifty-nine direct care staff and parents from the upper mid-west and northeast regions of the country completed a survey measuring the prevalence of major habit disorders (bruxism, trichotillomania, motor/vocal tics, and stuttering) and habit behaviors exhibited by individuals with mental retardation being served by community residential agencies. Habit disorders were also subjectively evaluated according to the respondents' perception of the severity and noticeability of the behavior, and naturalness of appearance of the individual. In addition, respondents indicated their usual reaction to the habit disorder. Overall, stuttering was found to be the most prevalent habit disorder (32%), followed by tics (16·6%), bruxism (13·1%), and trichotillomania (5%) for the sample surveyed. Furthermore, differences were found in the prevalence of habit disorders and other habit behaviors by level of mental retardation and age (adults versus children/adolescents). The implications and limitations of the data, as well as suggestions for future investigations of habit disorders and habit behaviors exhibited by individuals with mental retardation, are discussed. © 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献
156.
157.
V A Lumley R G Miltenberger E S Long J T Rapp J A Roberts 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1998,31(1):91-101
Programs to teach sexual abuse prevention skills to persons with mental retardation have rarely been evaluated empirically, and typical evaluations are limited to assessment of the participants' knowledge rather than their performance of specific skills. In the present study, 6 adult women with mental retardation were trained in sexual abuse prevention, and performance was assessed using four separate measures: pretests and posttests of knowledge, verbal report, role play, and naturalistic probes. All women learned the skills but failed to exhibit them to criterion during the probes. We discuss the implications for further training and assessment of sexual abuse prevention skills. 相似文献
158.
William S. Long Esther J. Long Gregory H. Dobbins 《Journal of business and psychology》1998,12(3):299-312
The present research examined the effects of performance level and individual differences on participants' satisfaction with a peer evaluation system. One hundred and fifty-three individuals working in groups completed a questionnaire assessing self-monitoring, self-esteem, and individualism. A peer evaluation system was used at the end of the project. Subjects indicated their satisfaction with and reaction to the peer evaluation system. The findings demonstrated that performance level, individualism, self-esteem, and self-monitoring helped to understand and to predict group members' reactions to the peer evaluation system. Implications for future research and implementation of peer rating systems are discussed. 相似文献
159.
160.