首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   585篇
  免费   30篇
  2019年   9篇
  2018年   12篇
  2017年   8篇
  2016年   9篇
  2015年   5篇
  2014年   11篇
  2013年   68篇
  2012年   18篇
  2011年   16篇
  2010年   8篇
  2008年   11篇
  2007年   22篇
  2006年   21篇
  2005年   14篇
  2004年   12篇
  2003年   17篇
  2002年   20篇
  2001年   11篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   14篇
  1998年   8篇
  1997年   8篇
  1996年   8篇
  1995年   10篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   9篇
  1991年   10篇
  1990年   11篇
  1989年   10篇
  1988年   8篇
  1987年   7篇
  1986年   13篇
  1985年   9篇
  1984年   12篇
  1983年   7篇
  1982年   8篇
  1981年   8篇
  1980年   8篇
  1979年   13篇
  1977年   7篇
  1976年   10篇
  1975年   12篇
  1974年   8篇
  1973年   10篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   5篇
  1968年   4篇
  1966年   8篇
  1959年   4篇
排序方式: 共有615条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
111.
Operant devices have been used for many years in animal behavior research, yet such devices a regenerally highly specialized and quite expensive. Although commercial models are somewhat adaptable and resilient, they are also extremely expensive and are controlled by difficult to learn proprietary software. As an alternative to commercial devices, we have designed and produced a fully functional, programmable operant device, using a PICmicro microcontroller (Microchip Technology, Inc.). The electronic operant testing apparatus (ELOPTA) is designed to deliver food when a study animal, in this case a bird, successfully depresses the correct sequence of illuminated keys. The device logs each keypress and can detect and log whenever a test animal i spositioned at the device. Data can be easily transferred to a computer and imported into any statistical analysis software. At about 3% the cost of a commercial device, ELOPTA will advance behavioral sciences, including behavioral ecology, animal learning and cognition, and ethology.  相似文献   
112.
113.
There are occasions in psychological research where it is desirable to have available sets of variables with arbitrary intercorrelations. A quite simple procedure is described for generating pairs of such variables.  相似文献   
114.
Kimura (1980, The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 240-241; 1983, Canadian Journal of Psychology, 37, 19-35; 1987, Canadian Psychology, 28, 133-147) recently proposed that there are intrahemispheric sex differences in the organization of particular language and praxic functions such that in females these functions are more focally represented. This hypothesis, as well as supporting data, was derived from research with brain-injured subjects. The purpose of the present study was to test the utility of using dual-task methodology for investigating sex-related variation in intrahemispheric functional organization in normal individuals. Women were found to show greater interference than men on concurrent tasks of language and right-hand finger tapping. In support of Kimura's model, the present findings yielded evidence suggestive of intrahemispheric sex differences in the "functional distance" between language and praxic functions within the left hemisphere of normal individuals.  相似文献   
115.
We investigated surface interpolation in displays of structure from motion (SFM). To do so, we introduced a new method for measuring surface perception in dynamic displays--the SFM probe. An SFM probe is a dot that moves rigidly with the dots on a simulated surface, and whose distance from that surface can be adjusted with a joystick or similar control. The displays we studied were random-dot cylinders containing a vertical strip devoid of feature points (the gap). Subjects adjusted an SFM probe, presented in the gap, until the probe dot appeared to be on the surface. Variability in probe-dot placement decreased with increasing texture density on the cylinder and increased with increasing gap width. Subjects showed a consistent bias to place the probe dot outside the cylinder. This bias increased with increasing texture density for the SFM displays. (The opposite bias was found in a static two-dimensional interpolation task with an arc whose curvature matched that of the cylinder: Subjects placed the probe dot inside the arc.) This outside bias is inconsistent with several theoretical approaches to surface interpolation.  相似文献   
116.
In Experiment 1 (N = 16), under conditions of high memory load (60 pictures and 50 paragraphs) and a 1-week retention interval, undergraduate subjects reported their memory for photographs of scenes (cued recall and free-recall tasks). Subjects frequently reported memory for photographs that they had actually never seen, but had read about in a brief paragraph. In Experiment 2 (N = 40), the same pattern of results was obtained with immediate testing. Experiment 2 also demonstrated that the likelihood of subjects falsely attributing scene memory (based on reading) to actually having viewed a photograph was reduced when metacognitive awareness of imaging during reading was made salient. Awareness of image creation was induced by requiring subjects to rate the paragraphs with respect to imagery vividness. Although other measures of memory remained the same, subjects in the induced-imagery condition made 50% fewer confusion errors than subjects who read the paragraphs without imagery instructions. The results are discussed in the context of Johnson and Raye's (1981) reality monitoring model.  相似文献   
117.
Multivariate clustering procedures were used to identify homogeneous subgroups of outpatient sex offenders against children (n=110)on the basis of the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI). Results indicated a five-subgroup solution. Two within-normal-limits and two clinically elevated profile subgroups replicated subtypes found in previous cluster analytic studies of child sex offenders. The fifth subgroup appeared similar in MMPI profile pattern and elevation to a previously identified subtype of rapists. Discriminant analyses showed that subgroups differed along dimensions of cognitive disturbance and sexual functioning. Furthermore, levels of psychopathology corresponded with levels of sexual pathology. Results are discussed with reference to previous MMPI cluster analytic studies of sex offenders and theories of sexual aggression.Portions of this paper were presented at the meeting of the Midwestern Psychological Association, 1992, Chicago, Illinois.  相似文献   
118.
A system for studying prepulse inhibition in the leopard frog is described herein. It entails a modification of an apparatus developed almost a century ago, by Yerkes, for his investigations of hearing in the frog. With the present system, a frog is restrained in a cylindrical vessel (constructed from a 35-mm film canister), and the canister is positioned in the apparatus so that the animal’s legs hang downward. A miniature solenoid is used to deliver a startle eliciting tap to the frog’s head, and the resulting leg movement is detected by an optical densitometer. In addition to providing a convenient way to restrain frogs, our system enabled us to, in part, replicate Yerkes’s basic procedures with these subjects. As did Yerkes, we found that an acoustic signal just prior to a tap on a frog’s head inhibits the leg movement that would otherwise be elicited.  相似文献   
119.
The purpose of this study was to demonstrate the potential usefulness of conjoint analysis for organizational research, as evidenced in estimating the compensating wage differential of the four-day workweek. Five hundred and seventeen employees of an insurance company on the four-day workweek rated their preferences for increments and decrements in compensation and vacation time on both a four-day and five-day workweek. The data were analysed via conjoint analysis. The results revealed the four-day workweek schedule had a positive compensating wage differential of approximately US$1161, or about 6% of the sample's annual salary. The findings were discussed in terms of the prospects of using conjoint analysis for organizational research, including means of data collection, stimulus construction, parameter estimation methods, reliability, validity and computer-interactive systems.  相似文献   
120.
Thirty-eight adults were assigned to three groups: posttreatment hyperthyroid (n=13), low euthyroid (n=12), and high euthyroid (n=13). Physiological and subjective responses to three anxiety-induction procedures—public speaking, personally distressing imagery, and threat of shock—were compared across groups. Although there were differences across stressors for physiological and subjective measures, group differences were not detected. Results are discussed with regard to the risk for residual anxiety following treatment for hyperthyroidism and the possible influence of thyroid function on the etiology or maintenance of anxiety.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号